Alexander, J. M., Carr, M., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1995). Development of metacognition in gifted children: directions for future research. Developmental Review, 15, 1–37.
Article
Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Barcelo, F., & Knight, H. (2002). Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia, 40, 349–356.
Article
Google Scholar
Bekci, B., & Karakas, S. (2006). Metacognition and executive functions: related or different concepts. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(3), 343.
Google Scholar
Butterfield, E. C., Nelson, T. O., & Peck, V. (1988). Developmental aspects of the feeling of knowing. Developmental Psychology, 24(5), 654–663.
Article
Google Scholar
Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: the role of representation activation and maintenance. Developmental Science, 11(3), 339–353.
Article
Google Scholar
Crone, E. A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Worm, M., Somsen, R. J. M., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Switching between spatial stimulus–response mappings: a developmental study of cognitive flexibility. Developmental Science, 7(4), 443–455.
Article
Google Scholar
De la Fuente, J., & Lozano, A. (2010). Assessing self-regulated learning in early childhood education: difficulties, needs, and prospects. Psicothema, 22(2), 278–283.
Google Scholar
Deak, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure?—Evidence from preschool children’s inferences about word meanings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 194–222.
Article
Google Scholar
Deloache, J. S., & Brown, A. (1987). The early emergence of planning skills in children. In J. Bruner & H. Haste (Eds.), Making sense: The child’s construction of the world (pp. 108–130). London: Methuen & Co. Ltd.
Google Scholar
Devilly, G. J. (2005). ClinTools Software for Windows: Version 4. www.clintools.com: Psytek Ltd.
Eme, E., Puustinen, M., & Coutelet, B. (2006). Individual and developmental differences in reading monitoring: when and how do children evaluate their comprehension? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 21(1), 91–115.
Article
Google Scholar
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage.
Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Beech, D. R., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Development, 37, 283–299.
Article
Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Flavell, E. R., & Grossman, J. B. (1997). The development of children’s knowledge about inner speech. Child Development, 68, 39–47.
Article
Google Scholar
Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A. C. (1998). Metacognition in educational theory and practice. Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Problem solving processes in children’s construction and representations of closed railway circuits. Archives de Psychologie, 17, 33–59.
Google Scholar
Keeney, T. J., Cannizzo, S. R., & Flavell, J. H. (1967). Spontaneous and induced verbal rehearsal in a recall task. Child Development, 38(4), 953–966.
Article
Google Scholar
Krebs, S. S., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Children’s strategic regulation, metacognitive monitoring, and control processes during test taking. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 325–340.
Article
Google Scholar
Lambert, B. (2001). Metacognitive problem solving in preschoolers. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 26(3), 24–30.
Google Scholar
Larkin, S. (2000). How can we discern metacognition in year one children from interactions between students and teacher. Paper presented at the ESRC Teaching and Learning Research Programme Conference.
Molenaar, I., van Boxtel, C. A. M., & Sleegers, P. J. C. (2010). The effects of scaffolding metacognitive activities in small groups. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1727–1738.
Article
Google Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2002). Active versus latent representations: a neural network model of perseveration, dissociation and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 255–265.
Article
Google Scholar
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 26). New York: Academic.
Google Scholar
Perrotin, A., Belleville, S., & Isingrini, M. (2007). Metamemory monitoring in mild cognitive impairment: evidence of a less accurate episodic feeling-of-knowing. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2811–2826.
Article
Google Scholar
Perrotin, A., Tournelle, L., & Isingrini, M. (2008). Executive functioning and memory as potential mediators of the episodic feeling-of-knowing accuracy. Brain and Cognition, 67, 76–87.
Article
Google Scholar
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In P. R. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation doi:http://lib.myilibrary.com?ID=92724.
Puustinen, M. (1998). Help-seeking behavior in a problem-solving situation: development of self-regulation. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 13(2), 271–282.
Article
Google Scholar
Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Explicit and implicit confidence judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: an eye-tracking approach. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 229–250.
Article
Google Scholar
Roebers, C. M., Schmid, C., & Roderer, T. (2009). Metacognitive monitoring and control processes involved in primary school children’s test performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 749–767.
Article
Google Scholar
Sangster, C. (2010). The role of self-regulation in motor learning: exploring the self regulated motor performance of children with developmental coordination disorder. PhD thesis, University of Cambridge, Cambridge.
Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Vise, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000). Developmental trends in children’s memory monitoring: evidence from a judgment-of-learning task. Cognitive Development, 15, 115–134.
Article
Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Knopf, M., & Sodian, B. (2009). Verbal memory development from early childhood to early adulthood. In W. Schneider & M. Bullock (Eds.), Human development from early childhood to early adulthood. New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Souchay, C., & Isingrini, M. (2004). Age related differences in metacognitive control: role of executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 56, 89–99.
Article
Google Scholar
Sperling, R. A., Walls, R. T., & Hill, L. A. (2000). Early relationships among self-regulatory constructs: theory of mind and preschool children’s problem solving. Child Study Journal, 30(4), 233–251.
Google Scholar
Taconnat, L., Raz, N., Tocze, C., Bouazzaoui, B., Sauzeon, H., Fay, S., et al. (2009). Ageing and organisation strategies in free recall: the role of cognitive flexibility. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(2–3), 347–365.
Article
Google Scholar
van Hout-Wolters, B., Simons, R., & Volet, S. (2000). Active learning: Self-directed learning and independent work. In R. Simons, J. van der Linden, & T. Duffy (Eds.), New learning (p. 16). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Google Scholar
van Someren, M. W., Barnard, Y. F., & Sandberg, J. A. C. (1994). The think aloud method: A practical guide to modelling cognitive processes. London: Academic.
Google Scholar
Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.
Article
Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1977). Preschoolers understanding of memory-relevant variables. Child Development, 48(4), 1720–1723.
Article
Google Scholar
Whitebread, D., Anderson, H., Coltman, P., Page, C., Pino Pasternak, D., & Mehta, S. (2005). Developing independent learning in the early years. Education, 3–13(33), 40–50.
Google Scholar
Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Pino Pasternak, D., Sangster, C., Grau, V., Bingham, S., et al. (2009). The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children. Metacognition and Learning, 4(1), 63–85.
Article
Google Scholar
Whitebread, D., Pino Pasternak, D., Bryce, D., Vuillier, L., Buettner, G., Perels, F., et al. (2011). Identifying and measuring self-regulation in young children: the development and validation of the CHILD observational instrument. Paper presented at the EARLI Biennial Conference, University of Exeter, UK.
Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 279–306). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated learning. In P. R. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts, & M. Seidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. Orlando: Academic.
Google Scholar
Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. (2003). Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children aged 5 to 17. Child Development, 74(3), 659–678.
Article
Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.). (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 1–137.
Article
Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In P. R. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation doi:http://lib.myilibrary.com?ID=92724.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2001). Theories of self-regulated learning—an overview and analysis. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 191–226). NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar