Advertisement

Mind & Society

, Volume 6, Issue 2, pp 189–209 | Cite as

Semiosis in cognitive systems: a neural approach to the problem of meaning

  • Eliano Pessa
  • Graziano Terenzi
Original Article

Abstract

This paper deals with the problem of understanding semiosis and meaning in cognitive systems. To this aim we argue for a unified two-factor account according to which both external and internal information are non-independent aspects of meaning, thus contributing as a whole in determining its nature. To overcome the difficulties stemming from this approach we put forward a theoretical scheme based on the definition of a suitable representation space endowed with a set of transformations, and we show how it can be implemented, in the case of a single agent, by a neural network architecture. Numerical experiments conducted on different instances of the latter show that similar representations are developed as a consequence of the fact that these instances are facing a similar semantic task. This allows to model social and environmental influences through a system of interacting agents, each described by a specific implementation of this model architecture.

Keywords

Semiosis Linguistic symbols Meaning Two-factor accounts Neural networks Symbol categorization Semantic processing Cross-system relations 

Notes

Acknowledgement

This article is a revised version of the paper presented at the Second European Conference Computing and Philosophy (E-CAP2004), held at the University of Pavia, Italy (June 3–5, 2004) and chaired by Lorenzo Magnani.

References

  1. Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Block N (1986) Advertisement for a semantics for psychology. In: French P, Euhling D, Wettstein H (eds) Midwest studies in philosophy, vol 10. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp 615–678Google Scholar
  3. Bunge M (1959) Causality. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  4. Chalmers DJ (1990) Syntactic transformations on distributed representations. Connect Sci 2:53–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chalmers DJ (1992) Subsymbolic computation and the chinese room. In: Dinsmore J (ed) The symbolic and connectionist paradigms: closing the gap. LEA, Hillsdale, pp 25–48Google Scholar
  6. Chalmers DJ (1993) Connectionism and compositionality: why Fodor and Pylyshyn were wrong. Philos Psychol 6:305–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Christiansen MH, Chater N (1992) Connectionism, learning and meaning. Connect Sci 4:227–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dretske F (1981) Knowledge and the flow of information. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  9. Eliasmith C (2000) How neurons mean: a neurocomputational theory of representational content. PhD Dissertation, Department of Philosophy, Washington University, St. LouisGoogle Scholar
  10. Escudero P, Boersma P (2002) The subset problem in L2 perceptual development: multiple-category assimilation by dutch learners of Spanish. In: Proceedings of the 26th Boston University conference on language development, Somerville, pp 208–219Google Scholar
  11. Field H (1977) Logic, meaning and conceptual role. J Philos 74:379–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fodor J (1987) Psychosemantics. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  13. Fodor J (1998) Concepts: where the cognitive theory went wrong. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Fodor J, Lepore E (1992) Holism: a shopper’s guide. Basil Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  15. Gasser M (1993) The structure grounding problem. In: Proceedings of the 15th annual conference of the cognitive science society. LEA, Hillsdale, pp 149–152Google Scholar
  16. Goldstone RL, Rogosky BJ (2002) Using relations within conceptual systems to translate across conceptual systems. Cognition 84:295–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Harman G (1982) Conceptual role semantics. Notre Dame J Formal Logic 23:242–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42:335–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hornik K, Stinchcombe M and White H (1989) Multilayer feedforward networks are universal approximators. Neural Netw 2:359–366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kruschke JK (1992) ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychol Rev 99:22–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Laakso A, Cottrell G (2000) Content and cluster analysis: assessing representational similarities in neural systems. Philos Psychol 13:47–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Miikkulainen R, Dyer MG (1991) Natural language processing with modular neural networks and distributed lexicon. Cogn Sci 15:343–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Softky WR, Koch C (1993) The highly irregular firing of cortical cells is inconsistent with temporal integration of random EPSPs. J Neurosci 13:334–350Google Scholar
  24. Stampe D (1977) Towards a causal theory of linguistic representation. In: French P, Euhling D, Wettstein H (eds) Midwest studies in philosophy, vol 2. University of Minneapolis Press, Minneapolis, pp 42–63Google Scholar
  25. Sun R (2000) Symbol grounding: a new look at an old idea. Philos Psychol 13:149–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Usher M (2001) A statistical referential theory of content: using information theory to account for misrepresentations. Mind Lang 16:311–334Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Fondazione Rosselli 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of PaviaPaviaItaly

Personalised recommendations