Skip to main content
Log in

Improving multiple document comprehension with a lesson about multi-causal explanations in science

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Relying on multiple documents to answer questions is becoming common for both academic and personal inquiry tasks. These tasks often require students to explain phenomena by taking various causal factors that are mentioned separately in different documents and integrating them into a coherent multi-causal explanation of some phenomena. However, inquiry questions may not make this requirement explicit and may instead simply ask students to explain why the phenomenon occurs. This paper explores an Activity Model Hypothesis that posits students lack knowledge that their explanation should be multi-causal and how to engage in the kind of thinking needed to construct such an explanation. This experiment, conducted on a sample of eigth grade students, manipulated whether students received a short 10-min lesson on the nature of scientific explanations and multi-causal reasoning. Students who received this causal chain lesson wrote essays that were more causally complex and integrated, and subsequently performed better on an inference verification test, than students who did not receive the lesson. These results point to relatively simple changes to instructions that can provide the support students need for successful multiple-document comprehension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. Including this measure as a covariate did not change any of the reported results.

  2. All but one had non-significant Fs < 1, while the statistic for the interaction for comprehension test scores was above 1 but still not significant, F(1,128) = 2.11, p = .15, ηp2 = .02.

References

  • Alexander, P. A., the Disciplined Reading and Learning Research Laboratory. (2012). Reading into the future: Competence for the 21st century. Educational Psychologist, 47, 259–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.722511

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anmarkrud, Ø., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2014). Multiple-documents literacy: Strategic processing, source awareness, and argumentation when reading multiple conflicting documents. Learning and Individual Differences, 30, 64–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barzilai, S., Mor-Hagani, S., Zohar, A. R., Shlomi-Elooz, T., & Ben-Yishai, R. (2020). Making sources visible: Promoting multiple document literacy with digital epistemic scaffolds. Computers & Education, 157, 103980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barzilai, S., & Strømsø, H. I. (2018). Individual differences in multiple document comprehension. In J. L. G. Braasch, I. Bråten, & M. T. McCrudden (Eds.), Handbook of multiple source use (pp. 99–116). Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bennington, J. B. (2009). The carbon cycle and climate change. Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaum, D., Wiley, J., Britt, M. A., & Griffin, T. D. (2017). Thinking about global warming: The effect of policy-related documents and prompts on learning about causes of climate change. Discourse Processes, 54, 303–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L., Bråten, I., & McCrudden, M. T. (Eds.). (2018). Handbook of multiple source use. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315627496

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L., Rouet, J. F., Vibert, N., & Britt, M. A. (2012). Readers’ use of source information in text comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 40, 450–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Braasch, J. L., & Salmerón, L. (2020). Reading multiple and non-traditional texts: New opportunities and new challenges. In Handbook of reading research, volume V (pp. 79–98).

  • Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., Strømsø, H. I., & Rouet, J.-F. (2011). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: Towards an integrated model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 48–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Britt, M. A. (2009). Trust matters: Examining the role of source evaluation in students’ construction of meaning within and across multiple texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 6–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Aglinskas, C. (2002). Improving student’s ability to use source information. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 485–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J.-F. (2012). Learning with multiple documents: Component skills and their acquisition. In M. J. Lawson & J. R. Kirby (Eds.), The quality of learning: Dispositions, instruction, and mental structures (pp. 276–314). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Braasch, J. L. G. (2013). Documents as entities: Extending the situation model theory of comprehension. In M. A. Britt, S. R. Goldman, & J.-F. Rouet (Eds.), Reading: From words to multiple texts (pp. 160–179). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Rouet, J. F., & Durik, A. M. (2018). Literacy beyond text comprehension: A theory of purposeful reading. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., & Sommer, J. (2004). Facilitating textual integration with macro-structure focusing tasks. Reading Psychology, 25, 313–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cerdán, R., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2008). The effects of tasks on integrating information from multiple documents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 209–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2000). Self-explaining expository texts: The dual processes of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 161–238). Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coiro, J., & Dobler, E. (2007). Reading comprehension on the Internet: Exploring the online comprehension strategies used by sixth-grade skilled readers to search for and locate information on the Internet. Reading Research Quarterly, 42, 214–257. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.42.2.2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gil, L., Bråten, I., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010a). Summary versus argument tasks when working with multiple documents: Which is better for whom? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(3), 157–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gil, L., Bråten, I., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010b). Understanding and integrating multiple science texts: Summary tasks are sometimes better than argument tasks. Reading Psychology, 31(1), 30–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. R., Braasch, J. L. G., Wiley, J., Graesser, A. C., & Brodowinska, K. (2012). Comprehending and learning from internet sources: Processing patterns of better and poorer learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(4), 356–381. https://doi.org/10.1002/RRQ.027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., Britt, M. A., & Salas, C. (2012). The role of CLEAR thinking in learning science from multiple-document inquiry tasks. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 5, 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Individual differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 36, 93–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinze, S. R., Wiley, J., & Pellegrino, J. W. (2013). The importance of constructive comprehension processes in learning from tests. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 151–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, A. J., Velazquez, M. N., Dawdanow, A., & Shipley, T. F. (2018). Sketching and summarizing to reduce memory for seductive details in science text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 899–916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, A. J., & Wiley, J. (2015). Reading an analogy can cause the illusion of comprehension. Discourse Processes, 52, 376–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G., Rasmussen, L., Lin, M. H., Hoffman, R. R., & Case, J. (2014). Influencing preferences for different types of causal explanation of complex events. Human Factors, 56, 1380–1400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, K. (2009). Comprehension of relations among controversial texts: Effects of external strategy use. Instructional Science, 37(4), 311–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Black, J., Keselman, A., & Kaplan, D. (2000). The development of cognitive skills to support inquiry learning. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 495–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Bigot, L., & Rouet, J. F. (2007). The impact of presentation format, task assignment, and prior knowledge on students’ comprehension of multiple online documents. Journal of Literacy Research, 39, 445–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linderholm, T., Kwon, H., & Therriault, D. J. (2014a). Instructions that enhance multiple-text comprehension for college readers. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 45, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linderholm, T., Therriault, D. J., & Kwon, H. (2014b). Multiple science text processing: Building comprehension skills for college student readers. Reading Psychology, 35, 332–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, A., & Alexander, P. A. (2017). Cognitive affective engagement model of multiple source use. Educational Psychologist, 52, 182–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, A., & Alexander, P. A. (2019). Toward an integrated framework of multiple text use. Educational Psychologist, 54(1), 20–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, A., Du, H., & Wang, Y. (2019). Understanding students’ conceptions of task assignments. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 101801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombrozo, T. (2007). Simplicity and probability in causal explanation. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 232–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magliano, J. P., McCrudden, M. T., Rouet, J. F., & Sabatini, J. (2018). The modern reader: Should changes to how we read affect research and theory? In M. F. Schober, D. N. Rapp, & A. M. Britt (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of discourse processes (2nd ed., pp. 343–361). Taylor and Francis. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315687384-18

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (1989). Models for understanding. Review of Educational Research, 59(1), 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., Bailing Lyu, L. H., & Kulikowich, J. M. (2021). Bridging inferences and learning from multiple complementary texts. Discourse Processes, 58(5–6), 529–548. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2021.1924586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. (2004). SERT: Self-explanation reading training. Discourse Processes, 38, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millis, K. K., Simon, S., & tenBroek, N. S. (1998). Resource allocation during the rereading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 26, 232–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Towards a theory of documents representation. In H. van Oostendorp & S. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 99–122). Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J.-F. (2006). The skills of document use: From text comprehension to web-based learning. Lawrence Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203820094

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J.-F., Britt, M. A., Mason, R. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using multiple sources of evidence to reason about history. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 478–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., Carlo, M. S., Dufrense, R., & Mestre, J. (1996). The assessment of levels of domain expertise while reading. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 373–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Naumann, J., García, V., & Fajardo, I. (2017). Scanning and deep processing of information in hypertext: An eye tracking and cued retrospective think-aloud study. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33(3), 222–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saux, G., Britt, M. A., Vibert, N., & Rouet, J.-F. (2021). Building mental models from multiple texts: How readers construct coherence from inconsistent sources. Language and Linguistics Compass, 15(3), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoor, C., Rouet, J. F., Artelt, C., Mahlow, N., Hahnel, C., Kroehne, U., & Goldhammer, F. (2021). Readers’ perceived task demands and their relation to multiple document comprehension strategies and outcome. Learning and Individual Differences, 88, 102018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2014). The content-source integration model: A taxonomic description of how readers comprehend conflicting scientific information. In D. N. Rapp & J. L. G. Braasch (Eds.), Processing inaccurate information: Theoretical and applied perspectives from cognitive science and the educational sciences (pp. 379–402). MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Britt, M. A. (2010). Reading multiple texts about climate change: The relationship between memory for sources and text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 20, 192–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (2018). Use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Research in Science Education, 48(6), 1273–1296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarchi, C., & Mason, L. (2020). Effects of critical thinking on multiple-document comprehension. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 35(2), 289–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Meter, P., & Garner, J. (2005). The promise and practice of learner-generated drawing: Literature review and synthesis. Educational Psychology Review, 17(4), 285–325. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-005-8136-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandermeulen, N., van den Broek, B., van Steendam, E., & Rijlaarsdam, G. (2020). In search of an effective source use pattern for writing argumentative and informative synthesis texts. Reading and Writing, 33(2), 239–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrantsidis, T. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2022). Simplicity as a cue to probability: Multiple roles for simplicity in evaluating explanations. Cognitive Science, 46(7), e13169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J. (2019). Picture this! Effects of photographs, diagrams, animations, and sketching on learning and beliefs about learning from a geoscience text. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 9–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3495

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Ash, I. K., Sanchez, C. A., & Jaeger, A. (2011). Clarifying readers’ goals for learning from expository science texts. In M. McCrudden, J. Magliano, & G. Schraw (Eds.), Text relevance and learning from text (pp. 353–374). Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Goldman, S., Graesser, A., Sanchez, C., Ash, I., & Hemmerich, J. (2009). Source evaluation, comprehension, and learning in internet science inquiry tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 46, 1060–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Griffin, T. D., Steffens, B., & Britt, M. A. (2020). Beliefs related to the value of corroboration and integration of evidence from multiple documents in history. Learning & Instruction, 65, 101266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Hastings, P., Blaum, D., Jaeger, A. J., Hughes, S., Wallace, P., Griffin, T. D., & Britt, M. A. (2017). Different approaches to assessing the quality of explanations following a multiple-document inquiry activity in science. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 27(4), 758–790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Jaeger, A. J., & Griffin, T. D. (2018). Effects of task instructions on comprehension from multiple sources in history and science. In J. L. G. Braasch, I. Bråten, & M. T. McCrudden (Eds.), Handbook of multiple source use. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., & Voss, J. F. (1999). Constructing arguments from multiple sources: Tasks that promote understanding and not just memory for text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 301–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wineburg, S., & Rosenweig, R. (2023, May 10). Investigate: How was the Scopes trial more complicated than a simple debate between evolutionists and creationists? Historical Thinking Matters. https://historicalthinkingmatters.org/scopestrial/index.html

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by Grant R305F100007 from the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education M. Anne Britt, Thomas D. Griffin, Jennifer Wiley. The authors thank Raluca Birza, Dylan Blaum, Tara Brown, Sarah Davis, Tim George, Morgan Hager, Peter Hastings, Karyn Higgs, Tegan Michl, Alia Mohammad, Ishan Patel, Carlos Salas, Brent Steffens, Andrew Taylor, Amy Thakkar, Bianca Trigo and Patti Wallace for their contributions to this research as well as the other members of Project READi. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute of Education Sciences or the U.S. Department of Education.

Funding

This research was supported in part by Grant R305F100007 from the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education to M. Anne Britt, Thomas D. Griffin, Jennifer Wiley. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute of Education Sciences or the U.S. Department of Education.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TDG, AJJ, MAB, and JW all made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work; TDG and AJJ oversaw data collection; TDG, AJJ, and JW contributed to analysis and interpretation of data; TDG, AJJ, JW, and MAB all made substantial contribution to initial and revised drafts. All authors have approved the submitted version and have agreed to be accountable for the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas D. Griffin.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Inquiry task instruction

All students were given this written instruction page that was also read aloud to them.

The primary purpose of reading in science is to understand the causes of scientific phenomena. This means your goal for reading is to understand how and why things happen. To reach an understanding of a new topic in everyday life, we often need to gather information from multiple sources. In today’s task your goal is to learn about the causes of global temperature changes from several documents. You will have to piece together important information across the documents to construct a good understanding. No one text will provide the answer. This task is interesting because you are the one making the connections across documents and coming up with an explanation. No author has already done the work for you. It is also important that you use information from the documents to support your explanation of the causes.

Your task is to use this set of documents to write an essay explaining how and why recent patterns in global temperature are different from what has been observed in the past. Be sure to use specific information from the documents to support your conclusions and ideas.

[Causal chain lesson occurred here, for half of the participants]

Appendix 2

Causal chain lesson

Before you begin your main task of writing an essay, I want to help you better understand what good causal explanations in science tend to look like. A lot of times when we think about why something happened, we tend to just think of one simple major thing that caused it to happen. For example, we might say, “It’s snowing because it’s cold out.” But that’s not a very good explanation, because it doesn’t tell us how the cold creates snow and it doesn’t explain why it isn’t always snowing every time it’s cold. Usually good explanations in science include the more specific steps that occur in a kind of long causal chain.

Let’s look at an example that you might be familiar with. How does energy get to our muscles? You might say “Food” and that’s a great start, but is that really an explanation? We know WHAT gives our body energy (food), but do we know HOW it gets that energy?

What about food? What do I need to do with it? I need to eat it. That’s important to know, because there are lots of things you can do with food. Now, we all know that we’re supposed to eat food. It seems so obvious that we wouldn’t bother to even say it if we were telling someone what gives us energy. But eating it is a very important part of the process. When we are trying to explain things in science, it’s important to be specific and include all steps of a process even if you think other people would already know them.

Ok, so we still don’t really have a good explanation yet. How does the energy in the food we eat get to our muscles? We digest the food in our stomachs. So now we should ask, how does that happen? Acids in our stomach breaks down the food until it is liquid and can pass into our intestines.

But we still are not finished explaining yet. Remember what our main question is that we are supposed to answer: “How does energy get to our muscles?” We’ve only explained how food becomes liquid in our intestines, so there must be more links in our causal chain. Does anyone have any ideas?

Special chemicals called enzymes break down food particles into very small and simple molecules that can be absorbed through the intestine walls. From there they enter into the blood stream where they get carried to the muscles.

We could go on and on describing even smaller steps, but at least now we have a much more complete causal explanation that connects food to the energy it gives our muscles.

Also, causal explanations might not be just a single chain. For example many foods, like bananas, contain potassium, which increases the enzymes in our intestines. This means that bananas can be added and will change the rest of the chain.

It’s not so important that every part of the explanation line up in a single chain, but that each part of the explanation is connected to other parts. When you are creating an explanation in science you should try to think about how the various pieces of information you are reading connect together in causal chains. No single text will give you all the information. To write your essays, you will need to find each of the important pieces in the different texts and make connections.

In addition, while delivering this lesson, a causal diagram was constructed on the board.

figure a

Appendix 3

Initial reading instructions (emphasis in underline and bold in the originals)

Identify Important Ideas condition:

In this activity you will read a set of documents. Later you will be asked to use this set of documents to write an essay explaining how and why recent patterns in global temperature are different from what has been observed in the past.

To help you write this essay, your task for the first part of today is to closely read the documents in this set. While reading, it may help you to underline, highlight, or paraphrase important ideas in each document. There is space for you to summarize paragraphs or the passage as a whole. You may also find it helps you make sense of the documents if you note connections across documents or questions that you have.

You can read the documents in any order you wish, but you should read the BLUE sheet first. This will give you background on what you are reading about. Please feel free to write on the documents and take notes.

You will be able to use this task and the documents when you write your essay, but for now you will focus on closely reading the documents.

Identify Possible Causes condition:

In this activity you will read a set of documents. Later you will be asked to use this set of documents to write an essay explaining how and why recent patterns in global temperature are different from what has been observed in the past.

Understanding how and why things happen usually requires thinking about how different scientific processes fit together into larger systems. So, before you write the essay, you need to understand some general background. Your task for the first part of today is to use this set of documents to find things that can cause changes in temperature.

You can read the documents in any order you wish, but you should read the BLUE sheet first. This will give you background on what you are reading about. Please feel free to write on the documents and take notes.

You will be able to use this task and the documents when you write your essay, but for now you will focus on finding possible causes in the documents.

Appendix 4

Off-task essays that did not address the prompt

Example 1

Recent studies show that temperature is changing due to global warming. Average global temperature can drop by as many as 10°. Several ice ages have occurred throughout the globes history. Giant ice bergs recently are malting because of global warming. When global warming happens all the ice or ice bergs start to melt if this happen all the penguins, polars bears they will die if humans on earth don’t go green and protect this planed. Scientists say that the sun will die in 2000 years from now. If that does happen we will only be able to see if we have flash lights or car lights in order to see outside. At night we will be able to see because of the moon that we have if that dies to then we will not be able to see at all outside when it is dark.

So please people stop global warming by not litering and also go green because if you do these to thing this planet will be the best living planet ever were human beings ever lived so please people stop global warming by throwing away garbage in a garbage can and don’t throw it on the floor.

Example 2

The Earth’s temperature has changed over the past years. Historical records of measurements from the weather stations date back to the late 1900s. Permanent ice at the North and South Poles is made from snow that has fallen over hundreds and thousands of years. From the hydrogen found in the ice cores, scientists are able to calculate what the average global temperature was in the past. During the ice ages, glaciers cover as much as a third of the Earth’s land surface. Average global temperature can drop by as many as 10°. Several ice ages have occurred throughout our planet’s history.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Griffin, T.D., Jaeger, A.J., Britt, M.A. et al. Improving multiple document comprehension with a lesson about multi-causal explanations in science. Instr Sci (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-023-09657-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-023-09657-1

Keywords

Navigation