Ames, G. J., & Murray, F. B. (1982). When two wrongs make a right: Promoting cognitive development through cognitive conflict. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 894–897. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.894.
Article
Google Scholar
Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2007). The effects of monological and dialogical argumentation on concept learning in evolutionary theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 626–639.
Article
Google Scholar
Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2009). The role of argumentation and explanation in conceptual change: Indications from protocol analyses of peer-to-peer dialogue. Cognitive Science, 33, 373–399.
Article
Google Scholar
Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2016). Argumentation for learning: Well-trodden paths and unexplored territories. Educational Psychologist, 51(2), 164–187.
Article
Google Scholar
Asterhan, C. S. C., Schwarz, B. B., & Cohen-Eliyahu, N. (2014). Outcome feedback during collaborative learning: Contingencies between feedback and dyad composition. Learning and Instruction, 34(4), 1–10.
Article
Google Scholar
Babai, R., Sekal, R., & Stavy, R. (2010). Persistence of the intuitive conception of living things in adolescence. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19(1), 20–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-009-9174-2.
Article
Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., & Schwartz, D. L. (1999). Rethinking transfer: A simple proposal with multiple implications. Review of research in education, 24(1), 61–100.
Article
Google Scholar
Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13(4), 273–281. https://doi.org/10.1037/e527352012-769.
Google Scholar
Chan, C., Burtis, J., & Bereiter, C. (1997). Knowledge building as a mediator of conflict in conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 15(1), 1–40. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1501_1.
Article
Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Eds.), Handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0605-8_20.
Chi, M. T. H., Roscoe, R., Slotta, J., Roy, M., & Chase, M. (2012). Misconceived causal explanations for “emergent” processes. Cognitive Science, 36, 1–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01207.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63(1), 1–49. https://doi.org/10.2307/1170558.
Article
Google Scholar
Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35(6), 623–654. http://chilab.asu.edu/papers/Chi_concpetualchangechapter.pdf.
Diakidoy, I. A. N., Kendeou, P., & Ioannides, C. (2003). Reading about energy: the effects of text structure in science learning and conceptual change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 335–356. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00039-5.
Article
Google Scholar
Diakidoy, I. A. N., Mouskounti, T., Fella, A., & Ioannides, C. (2016). Comprehension processes and outcomes with refutation and expository texts and their contribution to learning. Learning and Instruction, 41, 60–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1978). Individual and collective conflicts of centration in cognitive development. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9(1), 245–247. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420090110.
Google Scholar
Dunbar, K., Fugelsang, J., & Stein, C. (2007). Do naïve theories ever go away? Using brain and behavior to understand changes in concepts. Thinking with data. https://doi.org/10.1108/00251740810865085.
Google Scholar
Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Fugelsang, J., & Dunbar, K. (2005). Brain-based mechanisms underlying complex causal thinking. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1204–1213.
Article
Google Scholar
Gadgil, S., Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Chi, M. T. (2012). Effectiveness of holistic mental model confrontation in driving conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 22(1), 47–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.06.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning and Instruction, 17, 612–634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.008.
Article
Google Scholar
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of educational research, 77(1), 81–112. https://doi.org/10.3102/003465430298487.
Article
Google Scholar
Hill, H., Rowan, B., & Ball, D. L. (2005). Effects of teachers’ mathematical knowledge for teaching on student achievement. American Education Research Journal, 42(2), 371–406. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312042002371.
Article
Google Scholar
Howe, C., Tolmie, A., Duchak-Tanner, V., & Rattay, C. (2000). Hypothesis-testing in science: Group consensus and the acquisition of conceptual and procedural knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 10(4), 361–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(00)00004-9.
Article
Google Scholar
Jensen, M. S., & Finley, F. N. (1996). Changes in students’ understanding of evolution resulting from different curricular and instructional strategies. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(8), 879–900. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2736(199610)33:8<879:aid-tea4>3.0.co;2-t.
Article
Google Scholar
Jiménez-Aleixandre, M. P. (1992). Thinking about theories or thinking with theories?: A classroom study with natural selection. International Journal of Science Education, 14(1), 51–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069920140106.
Article
Google Scholar
Kapur, M., & Bielaczyc, K. (2011). Classroom-based experiments in productive failure. In L. Carlson, C. Holscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2812–2817). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. https://doi.org/10.1109/iecon.2007.4459876.
Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 254–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.254.
Article
Google Scholar
Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. L. C. (1988). Timing of feedback and verbal learning. Review of Educational Research, 58(1), 79–97. https://doi.org/10.2307/1170349.
Article
Google Scholar
Light, P., & Glachan, M. (1985). Facilitation of individual problem solving through peer interaction. Educational Psychology, 5, 217–225. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443418500503.
Article
Google Scholar
Limon, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: A critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4752(00)00037-2.
Article
Google Scholar
Loibl, K., & Rummel, N. (2014). Knowing what you don’t know makes failure productive. Learning and Instruction, 34, 74–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.08.004.
Article
Google Scholar
Masson, S., Potvin, P., Riopel, M., & Foisy, L. M. B. (2014). Differences in brain activation between novices and experts in science during a task involving a common misconception in electricity. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8(1), 44–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12043.
Article
Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S. (2002). Generating and understanding qualitative explanations. In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 91–128). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 301–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Özdemir, G., & Clark, D. (2007). An overview of conceptual change theories. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 3, 351–361.
Article
Google Scholar
Potvin, P., Masson, S., Lafortune, S., & Cyr, G. (2015a). Persistence of the intuitive conception that heavier objects sink more: A reaction time study with different levels of interference. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 13(1), 21–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-014-9520-6.
Article
Google Scholar
Potvin, P., Sauriol, É., & Riopel, M. (2015b). Experimental evidence of the superiority of the prevalence model of conceptual change over the classical models and traditional teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(8), 1082–1108. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21235.
Article
Google Scholar
Ramsburg, J. T., & Ohlsson, S. (2016). Category change in the absence of cognitive conflict. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(1), 98. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000050.
Article
Google Scholar
Richland, L. E., Kornell, N., & Kao, L. S. (2009). The pretesting effect: Do unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(3), 243. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016496.
Google Scholar
Sadler, P. M., Sonnert, G., Coyle, H. P., Cook-Smith, N., & Miller, J. L. (2013). The influence of teachers’ knowledge on student learning in middle school physical science classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 50(5), 1020–1049. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831213477680.
Article
Google Scholar
Schnotz, W., & Preuss, A. (1999). Task-dependent construction of mental models as a basis for conceptual change. In W. Schnotz, S. Vosniadou, & M. Carretero (Eds.), New perspectives on conceptual change (pp. 193–222). Amsterdam: Pergamon Press.
Google Scholar
Schwarz, B. B., & Linchevski, L. (2007). The role of task design and argumentation in cognitive development during peer interaction: The case of proportional reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 17(5), 510–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.009.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwarz, B. B., Neuman, Y., & Biezuner, S. (2000). Two wrongs may make a right… If they argue together! Cognition and Instruction, 18(4), 461–494. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1804_2.
Article
Google Scholar
Shtulman, A. (2006). Qualitative differences between naïve and scientific theories of evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 170–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.10.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124(2), 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Sinatra, G. M., & Broughton, S. H. (2011). Bridging reading comprehension and conceptual change in science education: The promise of refutation text. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(4), 374–393.
Article
Google Scholar
Tippett, C. D. (2010). Refutation text in science education: A review of two decades of research. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 8, 951–970.
Article
Google Scholar
Van Loon, M. H., Dunlosky, J., Van Gog, T., Van Merriënboer, J. J., & De Bruin, A. B. (2015). Refutations in science texts lead to hypercorrection of misconceptions held with high confidence. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 42, 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.04.003.
Article
Google Scholar
Vosniadou, S. (Ed.). (2009). International handbook of research on conceptual change. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203154472.ch1.
Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/nightcycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123–183.
Article
Google Scholar
Vosniadou, S., & Mason, L. (2013). Conceptual change induced by instruction: A complex interplay of multiple factors. In S. Graham, J. Royer, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Individual differences and cultural and contextual factors, Vol 2 of the APA Educational Psychology Handbook Series (pp. 221–246). APA Publications. https://doi.org/10.1037/13274-000.