Skip to main content
Log in

Testing the instructional fit hypothesis: the case of self-explanation prompts

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive science principles should have implications for the design of effective learning environments. The self-explanation principle was chosen for the current work because it has developed significantly over the last 20 years. Early formulations hypothesized that self-explanation facilitated inference generation to supply missing information about a concept or target skill, whereas later work hypothesized that self-explanation facilitated mental-model revision (Chi, Handbook of research on conceptual change, 2000). To better understand the complex relationship between prior knowledge, cognitive processing, and changes to a learner’s representation, two classes of self-explanation prompts (gap-filling and mental-model revision) were tested in the domain of physics problem solving. Prompts designed to focus the learner on gap-filling led to greater learning and a reduction in the amount of tutoring assistance required to solve physics problems. The results are interpreted as support for the instructional fit hypothesis—the idea that the efficacy of instruction is contingent on the match between the cognitive processing that the instruction elicits, how those processes modify the underlying knowledge representations for the task, and the utility of those representations for the task or problem.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aleven, V. A. W. M. M., & Koedinger, K. R. (2002). An effective metacognitive strategy: Learning by doing and explaining with a computer-based Cognitive Tutor. Cognitive Science, 26, 147–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., Corbett, A. T., Koedinger, K. R., & Pelletier, R. (1995). Cognitive tutors: Lessons learned. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(2), 167–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, R. S., Corbett, A. T., Koedinger, K. R., & Wagner, A. Z. (2004). Off-task behavior in the Cognitive Tutor classroom: When students “game the system”. Proceedings of ACM CHI 2004: Computer-Human Interaction, 383–390.

  • Berthold, K., Eysink, T. H., & Renkl, A. (2009). Assisting self-explanation prompts are more effective than open prompts when learning with multiple representations. Instructional Science, 37, 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catrambone, R. (1998). The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(4), 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapin, S., O’Connor, C., & Anderson, N. (2003). Classroom discussions: Using math talk to help students learn, grades 1–6. Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2000). Self-explaining expository texts: The dual processes of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 161–238). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three kinds of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and ontological shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), Handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., & Bassok, M. (1989). Learning from examples via self-explanations. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 251–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., DeLeeuw, N., Chiu, M.-H., & Lavancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis of the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conati, C., & VanLehn, K. (2000). Toward computer-based support of meta-cognitive skills: A computational framework to coach self-explanation. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 11, 398–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, R. G. M., & Chi, M. T. H. (2002). Can a computer interface support self-explaining? Cognitive Technology, 7(1), 4–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, R. G. M., & VanLehn, K. (2007). Explaining self-explaining: A contrast between content and generation. In R. Luckin, K. R. Koedinger, & J. Greer (Eds.), Artificial intelligence in education: Building technology rich learning contexts that work (Vol. 158, pp. 417–424). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keppel, G. (1991). Design and analysis: A researcher’s guide. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koedinger, K. R., Anderson, J. R., Hadley, W. H., & Mark, M. A. (1997). Intelligent tutoring goes to school in the big city. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 8, 30–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, D. P., O’Kuma, T. L., Hieggelke, C. J., & Van Heuvelen, A. (2001). Surveying students’ conceptual knowledge of electricity and magnetism. American Journal of Physics, 69(7), S12–S23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, S. P. (1995). Schemas in problem solving. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S. (2004). SERT: Self-explanation reading training. Discourse Processes, 38(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nokes, T. J., Schunn, C. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2010). Problem solving and human expertise. In P. Peterson, E. Baker, & B. McGraw (Eds.), International encyclopedia of education (Vol. 5, pp. 265–272). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2000). Measures for effect size for comparative studies: Applications, interpretations, and limitations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 241–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Bain, P., Bottge, B., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K., McDaniel, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing Instruction and Study to Improve Student Learning (NCER 2007–2004). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

  • Renkl, A. (2002). Learning from worked-out examples: Instructional explanations supplement self-explanations. Learning & Instruction, 12, 529–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. Child Development, 77, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B. H. (1984). Remindings and their effects in learning a cognitive skill. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 371–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B. H., & Kilbane, M. C. (1997). Effects of principle explanation and superficial similarity on analogical mapping in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(2), 427–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K. (1998). Analogy events: How examples are used during problem solving. Cognitive Science, 22(3), 347–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K., Lynch, C., Schultz, K., Shapiro, J. A., Shelby, R., Taylor, L., et al. (2005). The Andes physics tutoring system: Lessons learned. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence and Education, 15(3), 147–204.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation, Grant Number SBE-0354420 to the Pittsburgh Science of Learning Center (http://www.learnlab.org). No endorsement should be inferred. Portions of the results were presented at the Cognitive Science Society’s 31st Annual Conference. We would like to thank the members of the Cognitive Science Learning Laboratory and several anonymous reviewers for their many helpful comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy J. Nokes.

Appendix

Appendix

  Test problems and examples

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nokes, T.J., Hausmann, R.G.M., VanLehn, K. et al. Testing the instructional fit hypothesis: the case of self-explanation prompts. Instr Sci 39, 645–666 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-010-9151-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-010-9151-4

Keywords

Navigation