Abstract
This paper replicates and extends my earlier work on productive failure in mathematical problem solving (Kapur, doi:10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x, 2009). One hundred and nine, seventh-grade mathematics students taught by the same teacher from a Singapore school experienced one of three learning designs: (a) traditional lecture and practice (LP), (b) productive failure (PF), where they solved complex problems in small groups without any instructional facilitation up until a teacher-led consolidation, or (c) facilitated complex problem solving (FCPS), which was the same as the PF condition except that students received instructional facilitation throughout their lessons. Despite seemingly failing in their collective and individual problem-solving efforts, PF students significantly outperformed their counterparts in the other two conditions on both the well-structured and higher-order application problems on the post-test, and demonstrated greater representation flexibility in working with graphical representations. The differences between the FCPS and LP conditions did not reach significance. Findings and implications of productive failure for theory, design of learning, and future research are discussed.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
All inter-rater reliabilities reported in this paper are Krippendorff’s alphas.
References
Bielaczyc, K., & Kapur, M. (in press). Playing epistemic games in science and mathematics classrooms. Educational Technology.
Brown, A. L. (1992). Design experiments. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(2), 141–178.
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Carey, S. (1999). Sources of conceptual change. In E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, & P. Miller (Eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget’s legacy (pp. 293–326). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clifford, M. M. (1984). Thoughts on a theory of constructive failure. Educational Psychologist, 19(2), 108–120.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., & Yackel, E. (1993). Discourse, mathematical thinking and classroom practice. In E. Forman, N. Minick, & C. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 91–119). New York: Oxford University Press.
diSessa, A. A. (2006). A history of conceptual change research: Threads and fault lines. In K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
diSessa, A. A., Hammer, D., Sherin, B., & Kolpakowski, T. (1991). Inventing graphing: meta-representational expertise in children. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 10(2), 117–160.
Goldin, G. A. (2008). Perspectives on representation in mathematical learning and problem solving. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 176–201). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Janvier, C. (1987). Problems of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 379–424.
Kapur, M. (2009). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science. doi:10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x.
Kapur, M., Dickson, L., & Toh, P. Y. (2008). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1717–1722). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Kapur, M., Hung, D., Jacobson, M., Voiklis, J., Kinzer, C., & Chen, D.-T. (2007). Emergence of learning in computer-supported, large-scale collective dynamics: A research agenda. In C. A. Clark, G. Erkens, & S. Puntambekar (Eds.), Proceedings of the international conference of computer-supported collaborative learning (pp. 323–332). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kapur, M., & Kinzer, C. (2007). The effect of problem type on interactional activity, inequity, and group performance in a synchronous computer-supported collaborative environment. Educational Technology, Research and Development, 55(5), 439–459.
Kapur, M., & Kinzer, C. (2009). Productive failure in CSCL groups. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning (ijCSCL), 4(1), 21–46.
Kapur, M., & Lee, J. (2009). Designing for productive failure in mathematical problem solving. In N. Taatgen & V. R. Hedderick (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2632–2637). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Kapur, M., & Lee, J. (2010). Productive failure in learning the concept of variance. In R. Catrambone & S. Ohlsson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society (in press).
Kapur, M., & Rummel, N. (2009). The assistance dilemma in CSCL. In Proceedings of the computer-supported collaborative learning conference. Rhodes, Greece
Kapur, M., Voiklis, J., Kinzer, C., & Black, J. (2006). Insights into the emergence of convergence in group discussions. In S. Barab, K. Hay, & D. Hickey (Eds.), Proceedings of the international conference on the learning sciences (pp. 300–306). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86.
Koedinger, K. R., & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance dilemma in experiments with cognitive tutors. Educational Psychological Review, 19(3), 239–264.
Lesh, R. R., & Doerr, H. M. (2003). Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving, learning, and teaching. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pea, R. D. (2004). The social and technological dimensions of scaffolding and related theoretical concepts of learning, education, and human activity. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(3), 423–451.
Piaget, J. (1963). The psychology of intelligence. New York: Routledge.
Puntambekar, S., & Hübscher, R. (2005). Tools for scaffolding students in a complex learning environment: What have we gained and what have we missed? Educational Psychologist, 40(1), 1–12.
Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3(4), 207–217.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2008). Research methods in mathematics education. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 467–519). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16(4), 475–522.
Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 129–184.
Sweller, J. (2010). What human cognitive architecture tells us about constructivism. In S. Tobias & T. M. Duffy (Eds.), Constructivist instruction: Success or failure (pp. 127–143). New York, NJ: Routledge.
Tobias, S., & Duffy, T. M. (2010). Constructivist instruction: Success or failure. New York, NJ: Routledge.
Van Lehn, K., Siler, S., Murray, C., Yamauchi, T., & Baggett, W. B. (2003). Why do only some events cause learning during human tutoring? Cognition and Instruction, 21(3), 209–249.
Wadsworth, B. J. (1996). Piaget’s theory of cognitive and affective development. White Plains, NY: Longman.
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 17, 89–100.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendices
Appendix A: Student engagement survey
For the statements below, please use the five-point agreement scale and circle the number that most closely matches your opinion.
1. I enjoyed the lesson very much | 5 (SA) | 4 (A) | 3 (N) | 2 (D) | 1 (SD) |
2. I felt I was engaged in the lesson | 5 (SA) | 4 (A) | 3 (N) | 2 (D) | 1 (SD) |
3. I was attentive during the lesson | 5 (SA) | 4 (A) | 3 (N) | 2 (D) | 1 (SD) |
4. I participated in the lesson’s activities | 5 (SA) | 4 (A) | 3 (N) | 2 (D) | 1 (SD) |
5. I learned a lot in the lesson | 5 (SA) | 4 (A) | 3 (N) | 2 (D) | 1 (SD) |
Appendix B: Representational flexibility items
For examples of the well-structured and high-order application items, see Kapur (2009).
Tabular representation item: The property market has been on the rise for the past few years. In the newspaper, you find the following table with the growth rate over the past 5 years.
Year | % Growth |
---|---|
2003 | 2 |
2004 | 7 |
2005 | 11 |
2006 | 14 |
2007 | 16 |
Some people are saying that the property market is growing. Other are saying that it is slowing down. Based on the table above, what do you think—is the property market growing or slowing down? Explain your answer with calculations.
Graphical representation item: Bob drove 140 miles in 2 h and then drove 150 miles in the next 3 h. Study the two speed-time graphs A and B carefully. Which graph—A, B, or both—can represent Bob’s journey? Show your working and explain your answer.
Note: This item was adapted from Stanford Research International’s (SRI) research program on SimCalc and the Math of Change.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kapur, M. A further study of productive failure in mathematical problem solving: unpacking the design components. Instr Sci 39, 561–579 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-010-9144-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-010-9144-3