Instructional Science

, Volume 38, Issue 6, pp 523–550 | Cite as

Productive failure in mathematical problem solving

Article

Abstract

This paper reports on a quasi-experimental study comparing a “productive failure” instructional design (Kapur in Cognition and Instruction 26(3):379–424, 2008) with a traditional “lecture and practice” instructional design for a 2-week curricular unit on rate and speed. Seventy-five, 7th-grade mathematics students from a mainstream secondary school in Singapore participated in the study. Students experienced either a traditional lecture and practice teaching cycle or a productive failure cycle, where they solved complex problems in small groups without the provision of any support or scaffolds up until a consolidation lecture by their teacher during the last lesson for the unit. Findings suggest that students from the productive failure condition produced a diversity of linked problem representations and methods for solving the problems but were ultimately unsuccessful in their efforts, be it in groups or individually. Expectedly, they reported low confidence in their solutions. Despite seemingly failing in their collective and individual problem-solving efforts, students from the productive failure condition significantly outperformed their counterparts from the lecture and practice condition on both well-structured and higher-order application problems on the post-tests. After the post-test, they also demonstrated significantly better performance in using structured-response scaffolds to solve problems on relative speed—a higher-level concept not even covered during instruction. Findings and implications of productive failure for instructional design and future research are discussed.

Keywords

Ill-structured problems Failure in problem solving Persistence Classroom-based research Mathematical problem solving 

References

  1. Amit, M., & Fried, M. N. (2005). Multiple representations in 8th grade algebra classrooms: Are learners really getting it? In H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 2, pp. 57–64). Melbourne: PME.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
  3. Barron, B. (2003). When smart groups fail. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(3), 307–359. doi:10.1207/S15327809JLS1203_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown, A. L. (1992). Design experiments. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(2), 141–178. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0202_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1), 32–42.Google Scholar
  6. Bruner, J. S. (1985). Vygotsky: A historical and conceptual perspective. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 21–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Cho, K. L., & Jonassen, D. H. (2002). The effects of argumentation scaffolds on argumentation and problem solving. Educational Technology Research and Development, 50(3), 5–22. doi:10.1007/BF02505022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clifford, M. M. (1984). Thoughts on a theory of constructive failure. Educational Psychologist, 19(2), 108–120.Google Scholar
  11. Cohen, E. G., Lotan, R. A., Abram, P. L., Scarloss, B. A., & Schultz, S. E. (2002). Can groups learn? Teachers College Record, 104(6), 1045–1068. doi:10.1111/1467-9620.00196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dillenbourg, P. (2002). Over-scripting CSCL: The risks of blending collaborative learning with instructional design. In P. A. Kirschner (Ed.), Three worlds of CSCL. Can we support CSCL (pp. 61–91). Heerlen: Open Universiteit Nederland.Google Scholar
  13. Dixon, J. A., & Bangert, A. S. (2004). On the spontaneous discovery of a mathematical relation during problem solving. Cognitive Science, 28, 433–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Even, R. (1998). Factors involved in linking representations of functions. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 17(1), 105–121. doi:10.1016/S0732-3123(99)80063-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fishman, B., Marx, R., Blumenfeld, P., Krajcik, J. S., & Soloway, E. (2004). Creating a framework for research on systemic technology innovations. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(1), 43–76. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1301_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Ge, X., & Land, S. M. (2003). Scaffolding students’ problem-solving processes in an ill-structured task using question prompts and peer interactions. Educational Technology Research and Development, 51(1), 21–38. doi:10.1007/BF02504515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review, 62, 32–41. doi:10.1037/h0048826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Goel, V., & Pirolli, P. (1992). The structure of design problem spaces. Cognitive Science, 16, 395–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goldin, G. A. (2002). Representation in mathematical learning and problem solving. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 197–218). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Greeno, J. G., & Hall, R. P. (1997). Practicing representation: Learning with and about representational forms. Phi Delta Kappan, 78(5), 361–367.Google Scholar
  22. Greeno, J. G., Smith, D. R., & Moore, J. L. (1993). Transfer of situated learning. In D. K. Detterman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction (pp. 99–167). Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
  23. Hardiman, P. T., Dufresne, R., & Mestre, J. P. (1989). The relation between problem categorization and problem solving among experts and novices. Memory & Cognition, 17(5), 627–638.Google Scholar
  24. Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In H. Stevenson, H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan (pp. 262–272). New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  25. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how do students learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16(3), 235–266. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000034022.16470.f3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Janvier, C. (1987). Problems of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Jonassen, D. H. (2000). Towards a design theory of problem solving. Educational Technology Research and Development, 48(4), 63–85. doi:10.1007/BF02300500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kapur, M. (2006). Productive failure. In S. Barab, K. Hay, & D. Hickey (Eds.), Proceedings of the international conference on the learning sciences (pp. 307–313). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 379–424. doi:10.1080/07370000802212669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kapur, M., Dickson, L., & Toh, P. Y. (2008). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1717–1722). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  31. Kapur, M., & Kinzer, C. (2009). Productive failure in CSCL groups. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning (ijCSCL), 4(1), 21–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kaput, J. (1999). Representations, inscriptions, descriptions and learning: A kaleidoscope of windows. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 17(2), 265–281. doi:10.1016/S0364-0213(99)80062-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kohl, P. B., Rosengrant, D., & Finkelstein, N. D. (2007). Strongly and weakly directed approaches to teaching multiple representation use in physics. Physical Review Special Topics-Physics. Education Research, 3, 1–10.Google Scholar
  35. Lampert, M. (2001). Teaching problems and the problems of teaching. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Lesh, R. R., & Doerr, H. M. (2003). Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving, learning, and teaching. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  37. Lobato, J. (2003). How design experiments can inform a rethinking of transfer and vice versa. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 17–20. doi:10.3102/0013189X032001017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Marton, F. (2007). Sameness and difference in transfer. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(4), 499–535. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1504_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McNamara, D. S. (2001). Reading both high-coherence and low-coherence texts: Effects of text sequence and prior knowledge. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 51–62. doi:10.1037/h0087352.Google Scholar
  40. Mestre, J. (2005). Transfer of learning from a modern multidisciplinary perspective. Greenwich: Information Age.Google Scholar
  41. Puntambekar, S., & Hübscher, R. (2005). Tools for scaffolding students in a complex learning environment: What have we gained and what have we missed? Educational Psychologist, 40(1), 1–12. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4001_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Reiser, B. J. (2004). Scaffolding complex learning: The mechanisms of structuring and problematizing student work. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(3), 423–451. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1303_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (2003). Knowledge building. In J. W. Guthrie (Ed.), Encyclopedia of education. New York: Macmillan Reference.Google Scholar
  44. Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3(4), 207–217. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00029.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16(4), 475–522. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1604_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 129–184. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci2202_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Spiro, R. J., Feltovich, R. P., Jacobson, M. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1992). Cognitive flexibility, constructivism, and hypertext. In T. M. Duffy & D. H. Jonassen (Eds.), Constructivism and the technology of instruction: A conversation. NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  48. Tatar, D., Roschelle, J., Knudsen, J., Shechtman, N., Kaput, J., & Hopkins, B. (2008). Scaling up innovative technology-based mathematics. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 17, 248–286. doi:10.1080/10508400801986090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1982). Inquiry processes in program development. Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 103–118. doi:10.1002/1520-6629(198204)10:2<103::AID-JCOP2290100202>3.0.CO;2-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. VanLehn, K., Siler, S., Murray, C., Yamauchi, T., & Baggett, W. B. (2003). Why do only some events cause learning during human tutoring? Cognition and Instruction, 21(3), 209–249. doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI2103_01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Voss, J. F. (1988). Problem solving and reasoning in ill-structured domains. In C. Antaki (Ed.), Analyzing everyday explanation: A casebook of methods. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  52. Voss, J. F. (2005). Toulmin’s model and the solving of ill-structured problems. Argumentation, 19, 321–329. doi:10.1007/s10503-005-4419-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 17, 89–100. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zeitz, P. (1999). The art and craft of problem solving. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.National Institute of EducationNanyang Technological UniversitySingaporeSingapore

Personalised recommendations