Synthese

, Volume 189, Issue 3, pp 535–554 | Cite as

The subject of attention

Article

Abstract

The absence of a common understanding of attention plagues current research on the topic. Combining the findings from three domains of research on attention, this paper presents a univocal account that fits normal use of the term as well as its many associated phenomena: attention is a process of mental selection that is within the control of the subject. The role of the subject is often excluded from naturalized accounts, but this paper will be an exception to that rule. The paper aims to show how we might reinstate the subject into the act of attention, endorsing the ordinary notion that attention is a direction of the mind by the subject, rather than a mere occurrence or happening. To do so, it lays out the best work of phenomenology, psychology, and neuroscience on specifying the ordinary notion of attention and, in finding them individually wanting, combines them into a unified view that avoids the problems of each. In this way the paper presents a “how possible” account of the ordinary notion of attention, wherein attention is enacted by a subject.

Keywords

Attention Phenomenology Psychology Neuroscience Endogenous Exogenous Active Passive 

References

  1. Allport, A. (1989). Visual attention. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of Cognitive Science. The MIT Press, 631–682.Google Scholar
  2. American Heritage Dictionary. (2012). Attention. Accessed March 30, 2012, from http://ahdictionary.com/.
  3. Baluch F., Itti L. (2011) Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends in Neurosciences. 34: 210–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bisley J. W., Goldberg M. E. (2003) Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science 299(5603): 81–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Block N. (2011) Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 15: 567–575CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bradley F. H. (1886) Is there any special activity of attention? Mind. 11: 305–323Google Scholar
  7. Busse, L., Katzner, S., & Treue, S. (2008). Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 16380–16385.Google Scholar
  8. Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51, 1484–1525.Google Scholar
  9. Carrasco M., Ling S., Read S. (2004) Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience 7(3): 308–313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Colby C., Goldberg M. (1999) Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 22: 319–349Google Scholar
  11. Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3: 201–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. De Brigard F., Prinz J. (2010) Attention and consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 1: 51–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. de Fockert J. W., Rees G., Frith C. D., Lavie N. (2001) The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science 291(5509): 1803–1806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. de Vries W. A. (1988) Hegel’s theory of mental activity. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NYGoogle Scholar
  15. Desimone, R. (2009). Neural synchrony and selective attention. Proceedings of International Joint Conference on Neural Networks 683–684.Google Scholar
  16. Eimer, M., & Driver, J. (2001). Crossmodal links in endogenous and exogenous spatial attention: Evidence from event-related brain potential studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 497–511.Google Scholar
  17. Flanagan O. (1995) Consciousness and the natural method. Neuropsychologia. 33: 1103–1115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gregoriou G., Gotts S., Zhou H., Desimone R. (2009) High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 324: 1207–1210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hajcak, G., Anderson, B., Arana, A., Borckardt, J., Takacs, I., & George, M. (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex stimulation modulates electrocortical measures of visual attention: Evidence from direct bilateral epidural cortical stimulation in treatment-resistant mood disorder. Neuroscience, 170, 281–288.Google Scholar
  20. Hegel G. W. F. (1971) Philosophy of mind. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  21. Herrmann K., Montaser-Kouhsari L., Carrasco M., Heeger D. J. (2010) When size matters: Attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nature Neuroscience 13(12): 1554–1559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Husserl E. (1975) Experience and judgment. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, ILGoogle Scholar
  23. Husserl E. (2001) Concerning passive and active synthesis. Lectures on transcendental logic. Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  24. James W. (2010) The principles of psychology. H. Holt and Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  25. Jonides J. (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. Attention and Performance IX 9: 187–203Google Scholar
  26. Kahneman D. (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  27. Klein R. M. (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(4): 138–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Knudsen E. I. (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30: 57–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lamme V. A. F., Roelfsema P. R. (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences 23(11): 571–579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lavie N. (1995) Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology 21(3): 451–468Google Scholar
  31. Marshall G.D. (1970) Attention and will. The Philosophical Quarterly. 20: 14–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Milliken B., Tipper S. P. (1998) Attention and inhibition. In: Pashler H. (eds) Attention. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  33. Mole C. (2011) Attention is cognitive unison: An essay in philosophical psychology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  34. Noudoost B., Moore T. (2011) The role of neuromodulators in selective attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 20: 183–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Oxford English Dictionary. (2012). Attention. Accessed March 30, 2012, from http://www.oed.com/.
  36. Peelen M. V., Heslenfeld D. J., Theeuwes J. (2004) Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. Neuroimage 22(2): 822–830CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.Google Scholar
  38. Posner M. I., Cohen Y. (1984) Components of visual orienting. Attention and Performance X 32: 531–556Google Scholar
  39. Reynolds J.H., Heeger D.J. (2009) The normalization model of attention. Neuron. 61: 168–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rosen A. C., Rao S. M., Caffarra P., Scaglioni A., Bobholz J. A., Woodley S. J., Hammeke T. A., Cunningham J. M., Prieto T. E., Binder J. R. (1999) Neural basis of endogenous and exogenous spatial orienting: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11(2): 135–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Roskies A. (2010) How does neuroscience affect our conception of volition?. Annual Review of Neuroscience 33: 109–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Serences J., Yantis S. (2006) Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 10: 38–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shomstein S., Yantis S. (2006) Parietal cortex mediates voluntary control of spatial and nonspatial auditory attention. The Journal of Neuroscience. 26: 435–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Spratling M. W., Johnson M. H. (2004) A feedback model of visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16(2): 219–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Titchener E. B. (1908) Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. The Macmillan Company, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Titchener E. B. (1910) Attention as sensory clearness. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods 7: 180–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Trappenberg, T. P., Dorris, M. C., Munoz, D. P., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 256–271.Google Scholar
  48. Urban Dictionary. (2012). Attention. Accessed March, 30, 2012 from http://www.urbandictionary.com/.
  49. Wolfe J. M., Horowitz T. S. (2004) What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it?. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 495–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Yantis S. (1998) Control of visual attention. In: Pashler H. (eds) Attention. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Philosophy DepartmentUniversity of AntwerpAntwerpBelgium

Personalised recommendations