Aizawa K. (2010) Computation in cognitive science: It is not all about Turing-equivalent computation. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 41(3): 227–236
Article
Google Scholar
Andersen R. A., Essick G. K., Siegel R. M. (1985) Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230: 450–458
Article
Google Scholar
Andersen R. A., Mountcastle V. B. (1983) The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 3: 532–548
Google Scholar
Batterman R. (2002) Asymptotics and the role of minimal models. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 53(1): 21–38
Article
Google Scholar
Batterman R. (2009) Idealization and modeling. Synthese 169: 427–446
Article
Google Scholar
Bechtel W. (2002) Decomposing the brain: A long term pursuit. Brain and Mind 3: 229–242
Article
Google Scholar
Bechtel W. (2008) Mental mechanisms: Philosophical perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. Routledge, London
Google Scholar
Bechtel W., Abrahamsen A. (2005) Explanation: A mechanistic alternative. Studies in History and Philosophy of the Biological and Biomedical Sciences 36: 421–441
Article
Google Scholar
Bechtel W., Mundale J. (1999) Multiple realizability revisited: Linking cognitive and neural states. Philosophy of Science 66: 175–207
Article
Google Scholar
Bechtel W., Richardson R. C. (1993) Discovering complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Google Scholar
Bickle J. (1998) Psychoneural reduction: The new wave. Bradford/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Bogen J. (2005) Regularities and causality; generalizations and causal explanations. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C 36(2): 397–420
Article
Google Scholar
Bogen J. (2008) The Hodgkin–Huxley equations and the concrete model: Comments on Craver, Schaffner, and Weber. Philosophy of Science 75(5): 1034–1046
Article
Google Scholar
Brozović M., Abbott L. F., Andersen R. A. (2008) Mechanism of gain modulation at single neuron and network levels. Journal of Computational Neuroscience 25: 158–168
Article
Google Scholar
Bunge M. (1964) Phenomenological theories. In: Bunge M. (eds) The critical approach: In honor of Karl Popper. Free Press, New York
Google Scholar
Catterall W. A. (2000) From ionic currents to molecular mechanisms: The structure and function of voltage-gated sodium channels. Neuron 26(1): 13–25
Article
Google Scholar
Chance F. S., Abbott L. F., Reyes A. D. (2002) Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron 35: 773–782
Article
Google Scholar
Chemero A., Silberstein M. (2008) After the philosophy of mind: Replacing scholasticism with science. Philosophy of Science 75: 1–27
Article
Google Scholar
Churchland P. S. (1986) Neurophilosophy. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Churchland P. M. (2005) Functionalism at forty: A critical retrospective. Journal of Philosophy 102(1): 33–50
Google Scholar
Churchland P. S., Koch C., Sejnowski T. J. (1988) Computational neuroscience. Science 241: 1299–1306
Article
Google Scholar
Cole K. S., Moore J. W. (1960) Potassium ion current in the squid giant axon: Dynamic characteristic. Biophysical Journal 1: 1–14
Article
Google Scholar
Craver C. F. (2001) Role functions, mechanisms, and hierarchy. Philosophy of Science 68(1): 53–74
Article
Google Scholar
Craver C. F. (2006) When mechanistic models explain. Synthese 153: 355–376
Article
Google Scholar
Craver C. F. (2007) Explaining the brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Book
Google Scholar
Craver C. F. (2008) Physical law and mechanistic explanation in the Hodgkin and Huxley model of the action potential. Philosophy of Science 75: 1022–1033
Article
Google Scholar
Crick F. (1989) The recent excitement about neural networks. Nature 337: 129–132
Article
Google Scholar
Cummins R. (1975) Functional analysis. Journal of Philosophy 72: 741–765
Article
Google Scholar
Cummins R. (1983) The nature of psychological explanation. Bradford/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Cummins R. (2000) “How does it work?” vs. “What are the laws?” Two conceptions of psychological explanation. In: Keil F., Wilson R. (eds) Explanation and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 117–145
Google Scholar
Dawson M. R. W. (1998) Understanding cognitive science. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford
Google Scholar
Dayan P., Abbott L. F. (2001) Theoretical neuroscience: Computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Downes, S. M. (1992) The importance of models in theorizing: A deflationary semantic approach. In D. Hull, M. Forbes, & K. Okrulik (Eds.), Proceedings of the Philosophy of Science Association (Vol. 1, pp. 142–153). East Lansing.
Doyle D. A., Cabral J., Pfuetzner R. A., Kuo A., Gulbis J. M., Cohen S. L., Chait B. T., MacKinnon R. (1998) The structure of the potassium channel: Molecular basis of K
+ conduction and selectivity. Science 280: 69–77
Article
Google Scholar
Dray W. H. (1993) Philosophy of history. (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall, Englewood, NJ
Google Scholar
Dror I. E., Gallogly D. P. (1999) Computational analyses in cognitive neuroscience: In defense of biological implausibility. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 6(2): 173–182
Article
Google Scholar
Einevoll G.T. (2006) Mathematical modeling of neural activity. In: Skjeltorp A., Belushkin A.V. (eds) Dynamics of complex interconnected systems: Networks and bioprocesses NATO Science Series II: Mathematics, Physics & Chemistry. Kluwer, Amsterdam
Google Scholar
Einevoll G. T., Heggelund P. (2000) Mathematical models for the spatial receptive-field organization of nonlagged X-cells in dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of cat. Visual Neuroscience 17(6): 871–885
Article
Google Scholar
Einevoll G. T., Plesser H. E. (2002) Linear mechanistic models for the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of cat probed using drifting-grating stimuli. Network: Computation in Neural Systems 13: 503–530
Article
Google Scholar
Einevoll G. T., Plesser H. E. (2005) Responses of the difference-of-Gaussians model to circular drifting-grating patches. Visual Neuroscience 22(4): 437–446
Article
Google Scholar
Enroth-Cugell C., Robson J. G. (1966) The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat. Journal of Physiology 187(3): 517–552
Google Scholar
Ermentrout G. B., Terman D. H. (2010) Mathematical foundations of neuroscience. Springer, New York
Book
Google Scholar
Fodor J. A. (1974) Special sciences (Or: The disunity of science as a working hypothesis). Synthese 28: 97–115
Article
Google Scholar
Fodor J. A. (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Forber P. (2010) Confirmation and explaining how possible. Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in the History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 41(1): 32–40
Article
Google Scholar
Haugeland J. (1978) The nature and plausibility of cognitivism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 215–226
Article
Google Scholar
Hempel C. G. (1965) Aspects of scientific explanation and other essays in the philosophy of science. Free Press, New York
Google Scholar
Hille B. (2001) Ion channels of excitable membranes. (3rd ed.). Sinauer, Sunderland, MA
Google Scholar
Hille B., Armstrong C. M., MacKinnon R. (1999) Ion channels: From idea to reality. Nature Medicine 5(10): 1105–1109
Article
Google Scholar
Hodgkin A. L., Huxley A. F. (1952) A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. Journal of Physiology 117: 500–544
Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird P. N. (1983) Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge University Press, New York
Google Scholar
Kaplan, D. M. & Craver, C. F. (forthcoming): The explanatory force of dynamical and mathematical models in neuroscience: A mechanistic perspective. Philosophy of Science.
Kuffler S. (1953) Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. Journal of Neurophysiology 16(1): 37–68
Google Scholar
Machamer P., Darden L., Craver C. F. (2000) Thinking about mechanisms. Philosophy of Science 67: 1–25
Article
Google Scholar
MacKinnon R. (2004) Nobel lecture: Potassium channels and the atomic basis of selective ion conduction. Bioscience Reports 24(2): 75–100
Article
Google Scholar
Marr D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W.H.Freeman & Co. Ltd, San Francisco
Google Scholar
Marr D., Hildreth E. (1980) Theory of edge detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 207(1167): 187–217
Article
Google Scholar
Marr D., Ullman S., Poggio T. (1979) Bandpass channels, zero-crossings, and early visual information processing. Journal of the Optical Society of America 69(6): 914–916
Article
Google Scholar
Mastronarde D. N. (1992) Nonlagged relay cells and interneurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus: Receptive-field properties and retinal inputs. Visual Neuroscience 8(5): 407–441
Article
Google Scholar
Mauk M. D. (2000) The potential effectiveness of simulations versus phenomenological models. Nature Neuroscience 3(7): 649–651
Article
Google Scholar
Mazzoni P., Andersen R. A., Jordan M. I. (1991) A more biologically plausible learning rule applied to a network model of cortical area 7a. Cerebral Cortex 1: 293–307
Article
Google Scholar
Mel B. W. (1993) Synaptic integration in an excitable dendritic tree. Journal of Neurophysiology 70: 1086–1101
Google Scholar
Neher E. (1992) Nobel lecture: Ion channels for communication between and within cells. Neuron 8(4): 605–612
Article
Google Scholar
Newell A., Simon H. A. (1976) Computer science as an empirical enquiry: Symbols and search. Communications of the ACM 19: 113–126
Article
Google Scholar
Piccinini G. (2006) Computational explanation in neuroscience. Synthese 153: 343–353
Article
Google Scholar
Piccinini G. (2007) Computing mechanisms. Philosophy of Science 74: 501–526
Article
Google Scholar
Piccinini G. (2010) The mind as neural software? Revisiting functionalism, computationalism, and computational functionalism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 81(2): 269–311
Article
Google Scholar
Piccinini, G., & Craver, C. F. (2011). Integrating psychology and neuroscience: Functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese. doi:10.1007/s11229-011-9898-4.
Pouget A., Sejnowski T. J. (1997) Spatial transformations in the parietal cortex using basis functions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9(2): 222–237
Article
Google Scholar
Pouget A., Snyder L. H. (2000) Computational approaches to sensorimotor transformations. Nature Neuroscience 3(Suppl): 1192–1198
Article
Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1967). The nature of mental states (Reprinted from Mind, language, and reality, by Putnam, Ed., 1975, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.)
Pylyshyn Z. W. (1984) Computation and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Revonsuo A. (2001) On the nature of explanation in the neurosciences. In: Machamer P., Grush R., McLaughlin P. (eds) Theory and method in the neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, PA, pp 45–69
Google Scholar
Rodieck R. W. (1965) Quantitative analysis of cat retinal ganglion cell response to visual stimuli. Vision Research 5(11): 583–601
Article
Google Scholar
Rusanen A.M., Lappi O. (2007) The limits of mechanistic explanation in neurocognitive sciences. In: Vosniadou S., Kayser D., Protopapas A. (eds) Proceedings of the European cognitive science conference. Francis and Taylor, London
Google Scholar
Salinas E., Abbott L. F. (1996) A model of multiplicative neural responses in parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 90: 11956–11961
Article
Google Scholar
Salinas E., Thier P. (2000) Gain modulation: A major computational principle of the central nervous system. Neuron 27: 15–21
Article
Google Scholar
Salmon W. (1984) Scientific explanation and the causal structure of the world. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Google Scholar
Salmon W. (1989) Four decades of scientific explanation. University of Pittsburg Press, Pittsburg, PA
Google Scholar
Schaffner K. F. (2008) Theories, models, and equations in biology: The heuristic search for emergent simplifications in neurobiology. Philosophy of Science 75(5): 1008–1021
Article
Google Scholar
Sejnowski T. J. (2001) Computational neuroscience. In: Smelser N. J., Baltes P. B. (eds) Encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences. Elsevier, Oxford, pp 2460–2465
Chapter
Google Scholar
Shadmehr R., Wise S. (2005) Computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing: A foundation for motor learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Google Scholar
Shagrir O. (1998) Multiple realization, computation and the taxonomy of psychological states. Synthese 114: 445–461
Article
Google Scholar
Shagrir O. (2006) Why we view the brain as a computer. Synthese 153: 393–416
Article
Google Scholar
Shapiro L. A. (2000) Multiple realizations. Journal of Philosophy 97: 635–654
Article
Google Scholar
Smolensky P. (1988) On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–23
Article
Google Scholar
Von Eckart B., Poland J. S. (2004) Mechanism and explanation in cognitive neuroscience. Philosophy of Science 71: 972–984
Article
Google Scholar
Weber M. (2008) Causes without mechanisms: Experimental regularities, physical laws, and neuroscientific explanation. Philosophy of Science 75(5): 995–1007
Article
Google Scholar
Weisberg M. (2007) Three kinds of idealization. Journal of Philosophy 104(12): 639–659
Google Scholar
Woodward J. (2003) Making things happen. Oxford University Press, New York
Google Scholar
Zipser D., Andersen R. A. (1988) A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331: 679–684
Article
Google Scholar