Social Justice Research

, Volume 31, Issue 1, pp 1–22 | Cite as

Justice Sensitivity and Cooperation Dynamics in Repeated Public Good Games

  • Thomas Schlösser
  • Sebastian Berger
  • Detlef Fetchenhauer
Article

Abstract

It is frequently observed that despite individual incentives to free ride, humans decide to cooperate with each other to increase social payoffs. In the current research, we address the effects of individual differences in justice sensitivity on cooperation. Using incentivized repeated public good games, we find that individual differences in justice sensitivity—the ease of perceiving, remembering, and reacting to injustice from the perspectives of an observer, beneficiary, or perpetrator, but not victim—substantially predicts cooperation in the absence of a punishment option. In contrast, when costly punishment is allowed for, cooperation becomes strategic as it also aims at avoiding subsequent punishment. If such a sanctioning mechanism is in place, justice sensitivity no longer predicts cooperation. The results regarding the degree of cooperation as reaction to initial non-cooperation of one’s counterparts highlight the role of justice-concerning personality traits for the sufficient provision of public goods, as sanctioning institutions are not always possible, effective, or suitable.

Keywords

Social dilemma Public goods game Punishment Justice sensitivity Personality Heterogeneity 

JEL Classification

C71 C72 C73 C91 C92 D70 H41 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Simon Gächter for providing us with the original z-Tree files of the public good games; Sandra Kieser and Katharina Schneider for help in data collection; Mario Gollwitzer, Mattia Nardotto as well as Christoph Feldhaus for support regarding data analysis and presentation. Financial support by the German Research Foundation (DFG) is gratefully acknowledged by Schlösser and Fetchenhauer (Grant Nos. FE-1017/2-1, FE-1017/2-2) and all authors (CLER, Cologne Laboratory for Economic Research).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

Thomas Schlösser declares that he has no conflict of interest. Sebastian Berger declares that he has no conflict of interest. Detlef Fetchenhauer declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the National Research Committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Almakiasa, S., & Weiss, A. (2012). Ultimatum Game behavior in light of attachment theory. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 515–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arbak, E., & Villeval, M. C. (2013). Voluntary leadership: Motivation and influence. Social Choice and Welfare, 40(3), 635–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Au, W. T., & Komorita, S. S. (2002). Effects of initial choices in the prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 343–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Balliet, D., Parks, C., & Joireman, J. (2009). Social value orientation and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12(4), 533–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumert, A., Halmburger, A., & Schmitt, M. (2013). Interventions against norm violations: Dispositional determinants of self-reported and real moral courage. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(8), 1053–1068.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baumert, A., Schlösser, T., & Schmitt, M. (2014). Economic Games: A performance-based assessment of fairness and altruism. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 30, 178–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blanco, M., Engelmann, D., & Normann, H. T. (2011). A within-subject analysis of other-regarding preferences. Games and Economic Behavior, 72, 321–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press: Princeton.Google Scholar
  9. Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish?: Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social issues, 31(3), 137–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eriksson, K., Strimling, P., & Ehn, M. (2013). Ubiquity and efficiency of restrictions on informal punishment rights. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 17–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000a). Fairness and retaliation: The economics of reciprocity. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 14(3), 159–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000b). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980–994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137–140.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Fetchenhauer, D., & Huang, X. (2004). Justice sensitivity and distributive decisions in experimental games. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(5), 1015–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fischbacher, U. (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71(3), 397–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gächter, S., Nosenzo, D., Renner, E., & Sefton, M. (2010). Who makes a good leader? Cooperativeness, optimism, and leading by example. Economic Inquiry, 50(4), 953–967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gollwitzer, M., & Rothmund, T. (2011). What exactly are victim-sensitive persons sensitive to? Journal of Research in Personality, 45(5), 448–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gollwitzer, M., Rothmund, T., Pfeiffer, A., & Ensenbach, C. (2009). Why and when justice sensitivity leads to pro-and antisocial behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 999–1005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gollwitzer, M., Schmitt, M., Schalke, R., Maes, J., & Baer, A. (2005). Asymmetrical effects of justice sensitivity perspectives on prosocial and antisocial behavior. Social Justice Research, 18(2), 183–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Greiner, B. (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: Organizing experiments with ORSEE. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1(1), 114–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gürerk, Ö., Irlenbusch, B., & Rockenbach, B. (2006). The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science, 312(5770), 108–111.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Güth, W. (1995). On ultimatum bargaining experiments: A personal review. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 27(3), 329–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hammond, R. G., & Morrill, T. (2016). Personality traits and bidding behavior in competing auctions. Journal of Economic Psychology, 57, 39–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162, 1243–1248.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Herrmann, B., Thöni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 1362–1367.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Hilbig, B. E., Zettler, I., & Heydasch, T. (2011). Personality, punishment and public goods: Strategic shifts towards cooperation as a matter of dispositional honesty–humility. European Journal of Personality, 26(3), 245–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hilbig, B. E., Zettler, I., Leist, F., & Heydasch, T. (2013). It takes two: Honesty–humility and agreeableness differentially predict active versus reactive cooperation. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 598–603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., & Miles, E. W. (1987). A new perspective on equity theory: The equity sensitivity construct. Academy of Management Review, 12, 222–234.Google Scholar
  32. Kurzban, R., & Houser, D. (2001). Individual differences in cooperation in a circular public goods game. European Journal of Personality, 15, 37–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lerner, M. J. (1981). The justice motive in human relations. In The justice motive in social behavior (pp. 11–35). Springer.Google Scholar
  34. Levati, M. V., Sutter, M., & Van Der Heijden, E. (2007). Leading by example in a public goods experiment with heterogeneity and incomplete information. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 51(5), 793–818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lotz, S., Baumert, A., Schlösser, T., Gresser, F., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2011a). Individual differences in third-party interventions: How justice sensitivity shapes altruistic punishment. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 4(4), 297–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lotz, S., Okimoto, T. G., Schlösser, T., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2011b). Punitive versus compensatory reactions to injustice: Emotional antecedents to third-party interventions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 477–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lotz, S., Schlösser, T., Cain, D. M., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2013). The (in)stability of social preferences: Using justice sensitivity to predict when altruism collapses. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 141–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lovaš, L., & Wolt, R. (2002). Sensitivity to injustice in the context of some personality traits. Studia Psychologica, 44, 125–131.Google Scholar
  39. Marwell, G., & Ames, R. E. (1979). Experiments on the provision of public goods. I. Resources, interest, group size, and the free-rider problem. The American Journal of Sociology, 84(6), 1335–1360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mikula, G., Petri, B., & Tanzer, N. (1990). What people regard as unjust: Types and structures of everyday experiences of injustice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20(2), 133–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80(4), 252–283.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102(2), 246–268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Gardner, R. (1992). Covenants with and without a sword: Self-governance is possible. The American Political Science Review, 86, 404–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Préget, R., Nguyen-Van, P., & Willinger, M. (2016). Who are the voluntary leaders? Experimental evidence from a sequential contribution game. Theory and Decision, 81(4), 581–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schmitt, M., Baumert, A., Gollwitzer, M., & Maes, J. (2010). The justice sensitivity inventory: Factorial validity, location in the personality facet space, demographic pattern, and normative data. Social Justice Research, 23(2), 211–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schmitt, M., Eid, M., & Maes, J. (2003). Synergistic person x situation interaction in distributive justice behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(1), 141–147.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Schmitt, M., Gollwitzer, M., Maes, J., & Arbach, D. (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schmitt, M. J., Neumann, R., & Montada, L. (1995). Dispositional sensitivity to befallen injustice. Social Justice Research, 8(4), 385–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Snyder, M., & Ickes, W. (1985). Personality and social behavior. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology. Special fields and applications (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 883–948). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  51. Stavrova, O., & Schlösser, T. (2015). Solidarity and social justice: Effect of individual differences in justice sensitivity on solidary behavior. European Journal of Personality, 29(1), 2–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stavrova, O., Schlösser, T., & Baumert, A. (2013). Life satisfaction and job‐seeking behavior of the unemployed: The effect of individual differences in justice sensitivity. Applied Psychology: An International Review.  https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12009.Google Scholar
  53. Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2000). Cooperation in groups: Procedural justice, social identity, and behavioral engagement. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  54. Van Lange, P. A. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 337–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Effects of justice conditions on discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wijen, R., & van den Bos, K. (2010). Toward a better understanding of the justice judgment process: The influence of fair and unfair events on state justice sensitivity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1294–1301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(1), 110–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas Schlösser
    • 1
  • Sebastian Berger
    • 2
  • Detlef Fetchenhauer
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute for Sociology and Social PsychologyUniversity of CologneCologneGermany
  2. 2.Institute of Organization and Human Resource ManagementUniversity of BernBernSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations