Abstract
Drawing upon emotions as social information theory, we propose that others’ emotions can influence individuals’ justice judgments, outcome satisfaction, and behaviors even when individuals are not unfairly treated themselves and in the absence of explicit information about the fairness of others’ treatment. Study 1 demonstrated that individuals make inferences about the outcome favorability and procedural justice encountered by others based on others’ expressions of guilt and anger, which also influence individuals’ judgments of others’ overall justice and outcome satisfaction. Studies 2 and 3 demonstrated that others’ emotions can influence individuals’ own judgments of procedural justice and overall justice. Specifically, individuals perceive lower levels of justice when another person expresses guilt or anger relative to no emotion. Moreover, others’ emotions influence individuals’ outcome satisfaction and behaviors (i.e., helping intentions and retaliation); these effects are mediated by individuals’ own justice judgments (i.e., procedural and overall justice). Theoretical implications related to the role of emotions as antecedents to justice judgments, the social function of emotions, and the impact of emotions on third-party observers are discussed.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Affective reactions may also occur in the form of complementary emotions (e.g., fear in response to anger; compassion in response to guilt). However, these reactions have only been found in exceptional cases (e.g., when anger was expressed by a high-power individual; Lelieveld, Van Dijk, Van Beest, & Van Kleef, 2012). We measured participants’ fear and compassion in Study 3. However, participants’ own fear in the anger condition and their own compassion in the guilt condition did not significantly differ from the control condition.
Given that positive emotions have not been found to be associated with specific judgments of procedural justice (Krehbiel & Cropanzano, 2000; Weiss et al., 1999), we did not expect individuals to infer procedural justice information from another person’s positive emotions. Nonetheless, we included a pride condition in Studies 1 and 2 in an exploratory fashion. As expected, procedural and overall justice judgments in the pride condition did not significantly differ from the control condition in either study. We return to this point in the “General Discussion” section.
A potential exception is when individuals may need social comparison information to determine the favorability of their outcomes as compared to others. This point is further addressed in the “General Discussion” section.
Given that a female actor was used, potential gender effects were explored. Gender did not significantly moderate the relationships between condition and procedural or overall justice judgments. Female participants (M = 3.92, SD = .56) did judge their outcomes to be significantly more favorable than male participants (M = 3.54, SD = .77; F(1, 63) = 5.17, p < .05). However, controlling for gender did not substantively change the effect of outcome favorability on outcome satisfaction. No significant differences on performance were observed and controlling for gender did not substantively change the effects of overall justice and outcome favorability on performance.
Given that we asked participants to be “fast and accurate” for the Round 2 anagrams, it is possible that slower and/or less accurate responses could reflect retaliation. Further, rather than slowing down, some participants may have wanted to retaliate by speeding up their responses (e.g., to decrease accuracy). Accordingly, we explored participants’ accuracy on the task. Results indicated that the effect of condition on accuracy was non-significant, F(2, 61) = 2.86, p > .05, η² = .09. This suggests that individuals were inclined to retaliate by slowing down rather than being inaccurate.
We also conducted this analysis with all of the negative emotion items from our measure of participants’ own emotions (i.e., guilty, angry, afraid, anxious, embarrassed, envious, disgusted) collapsed into a general measure of the individual’s own negative emotions (α = .77). Condition did not significantly predict these negative emotions (F(2, 62) = 1.32, p > .05, η² = .04) and controlling for this general category did not substantively change the results.
Although we focused on others’ emotions as antecedents to one’s justice judgments and the potential influence of emotional contagion, it is important to note that emotions can also be outcomes of justice judgments (e.g., perceiving unfairness can prompt feelings of anger). Moreover, one of the key distinguishing features of emotions is that they have a target (i.e., emotions are directed at someone or something; Frijda, 1993). Future research may wish to further disentangle the influence of emotions by examining how emotions targeted toward the other’s treatment influence emotions targeted at one’s own justice judgment. For example, perhaps witnessing another’s anger or feeling angry about another’s treatment not only impacts one’s justice judgment but also the emotions emanating from this judgment (e.g., by intensifying or even changing the emotions that are experienced). Thus, it is important to further investigate the impact of emotions as antecedents, accompaniments, and outcomes of justice judgments as well as the interplay between emotions with different targets.
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this insight.
References
Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York: Academic Press.
Ambrose, M. L., & Schminke, M. (2009). The role of overall justice judgments in organizational justice research: A test of mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 491–500. doi:10.1037/a0013203.
Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24, 452–471. doi:10.5465/AMR.1999.2202131.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1968). Experimentation in social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 1–78). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Barclay, L. J., Bashshur, M. R., & Fortin, M. (2017). Motivated cognition and fairness: Insights, integration, and creating a path forward. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102, 867–889. doi:10.1037/apl0000204.
Barclay, L. J., & Kiefer, T. (2014). Approach or avoid? Exploring overall justice and the differential effects of positive and negative emotions. Journal of Management, 40, 1857–1898. doi:10.1177/0149206312441833.
Barclay, L. J., Skarlicki, D. P., & Pugh, S. D. (2005). Exploring the role of emotions in injustice perceptions and retaliation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 629–643. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.4.629.
Barsky, A., Kaplan, S. A., & Beal, D. J. (2011). Just feelings? The role of affect in the formation of organizational fairness judgments. Journal of Management, 37, 248–279. doi:10.1177/0149206310376325.
Bies, R. J. (1987). The predicament of injustice: The management of moral outrage. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 289–319). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. (2002). Hot flashes and open wounds: Injustice and the tyranny of its emotions. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, & D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on managing organizational justice (pp. 203–221). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Blader, S. L., Wiesenfeld, B. M., Fortin, M., & Wheeler-Smith, S. L. (2013). Fairness lies in the heart of the beholder: How the social emotions of third parties influence reactions to injustice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121, 62–80. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.12.004.
Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.
Brockner, J. (2010). A contemporary look at organizational justice: Multiplying insult times injury. New York: Routledge.
Brockner, J., Grover, S., Reed, T., DeWitt, R., & O’Malley, M. (1987). Survivors’ reactions to layoffs: We get by with a little help from our friends. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32, 526–541. doi:10.2307/2392882.
Carlsmith, J. M., Ellsworth, P., & Aronson, E. (1976). Methods of research in social psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Choi, J. (2008). Event justice perceptions and employees’ reactions: Perceptions of social entity justice as a moderator. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 513–528. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.513.
Chui, C. W., & Dietz, J. (2014). Observing workplace incivility towards women: The roles of target reactions, actor motives, and actor-target relationships. Sex Roles, 71, 95–108. doi:10.1007/s11199-014-0367-7.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen-Charash, Y., & Byrne, Z. S. (2008). Affect and justice: Current knowledge and future directions. In N. M. Ashkanasy & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Research companion to emotions in organizations (pp. 360–391). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.
Cohen-Charash, Y., & Spector, P. (2001). The role of justice in organizations: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 278–321. doi:10.1006/obhd.2001.2958.
Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386–400. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.386.
Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425–445. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.425.
Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., Rodell, J. B., Long, D. M., Zapata, C. P., Conlon, D. E., et al. (2013). Justice at the millennium, a decade later: A meta-analytic test of social exchange and affect-based perspectives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 199–236. doi:10.1037/a0031757.
Cropanzano, R., & Mitchell, M. S. (2005). Social exchange theory: An interdisciplinary review. Journal of Management, 31, 874–900. doi:10.1177/0149206305279602.
Cropanzano, R., Stein, J. H., & Nadisic, T. (2011). Social justice and the experience of emotions. New York: Routledge.
De Cremer, D., & Van Hiel, A. (2006). Effects of another person’s fair treatment on one’s own emotions and behaviors: The moderating role of how much the other cares for you. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100, 231–249. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.002.
De Cremer, D., & Van Hiel, A. (2010). “Becoming angry when another is treated fairly”: On understanding when own and other’s fair treatment influences negative reactions. British Journal of Management, 21, 280–298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00653.x.
De Cremer, D., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). When being overpaid makes me feel good about myself: It depends on how the other feels. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 793–802. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.05.008.
De Cremer, D., Wubben, M. J. J., & Brebels, L. (2008). When unfair treatment leads to anger: The effects of other people’s emotions and ambiguous unfair procedures. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 2518–2549. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00402.x.
De Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 73–88. doi:10.1037/a0034251.
Degoey, P. (2000). Contagious justice: Exploring the social construction of justice in organizations. In B. M. Staw & R. I. Sutton (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 22, pp. 51–102). New York: JAI Press.
Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organization justice (pp. 1–55). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.
Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 53–60. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.53.
Fortin, M., Nadisic, T., Bell, C. M., Crawshaw, J. R., & Cropanzano, R. (2016). Beyond the particular and universal: Dependence, independence, and interdependence of context, justice, and ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 137, 639–647. doi:10.1007/s10551-015-2823-x.
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes and emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381–403). New York: Guilford Press.
Glomb, T. M. (2002). Workplace anger and aggression: Informing conceptual models with data from specific encounters. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 20–36. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.7.1.20.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hillebrandt, A., & Barclay, L. J. (2013). Integrating organizational justice and affect: New insights, challenges, and opportunities. Social Justice Research, 26, 513–531. doi:10.1007/s11211-013-0193-z.
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203–253. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60415-3.
Jones, D. A., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2005). The effects of overhearing peers discuss an authority’s fairness reputation on reactions to subsequent treatment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 363–372. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.2.363.
Jones, D. A., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2013). How perceptions of fairness can change: A dynamic model of organizational justice. Organizational Psychology Review, 3, 138–160. doi:10.1177/2041386612461665.
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–521. doi:10.1080/026999399379168.
Kim, T.-Y., & Leung, K. (2007). Forming and reacting to overall fairness: A cross-cultural comparison. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 83–95. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.004.
Krehbiel, P. J., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). Procedural justice, outcome favorability and emotion. Social Justice Research, 13, 339–360. doi:10.1023/A:1007670909889.
Lamertz, K. (2002). The social construction of fairness: Social influence and sense making in organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 19–37. doi:10.1002/job.128.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lelieveld, G. J., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Why anger and disappointment affect other’s bargaining behavior differently: The moderating role of power and the mediating role of reciprocal and complementary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1209–1221. doi:10.1177/0146167212446938.
Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–55). New York: Plenum Press.
Lind, E. A. (2001a). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 56–88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Lind, E. A. (2001b). Thinking critically about justice judgments. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 220–226. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2001.1793.
Lind, E. A., Kray, L., & Thompson, L. (1998). The social construction of injustice: Fairness judgments in response to own and others’ unfair treatment by authorities. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75, 1–22. doi:10.1006/obhd.1998.2785.
Lind, E. A., & Van den Bos, K. (2002). When fairness works: Toward a general theory of uncertainty management. In B. M. Staw & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 24, pp. 181–223). Boston: JAI Press.
Manstead, A. S. R. (1991). Emotion in social life. In A. S. R. Manstead (Ed.), Emotion in social life (pp. 353–362). London: Erlbaum.
Mikula, G. (1986). The experience of injustice: Toward a better understanding of its phenomenology. In H. W. Bierhoff, R. L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations (pp. 103–123). New York: Plenum Press.
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50. doi:10.1080/02699938708408362.
Peters, S. L., & Van den Bos, K. (2008). When fairness is especially important: Reactions to being inequitably paid in communal relationships. Social Justice Research, 21, 86–105. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0056-6.
Peters, S. L., Van den Bos, K., & Bobocel, D. R. (2004). The moral superiority effect: Self versus other differences in satisfaction with being overpaid. Social Justice Research, 17, 257–273. doi:10.1023/B:SORE.0000041293.24615.f7.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 870–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.
Rodell, J. B., & Colquitt, J. A. (2009). Looking ahead in times of uncertainty: The role of anticipatory justice in an organizational change context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 989–1002. doi:10.1037/a0015351.
Rupp, D. E., Shao, R., Jones, K. S., & Liao, H. (2014). The utility of a multifoci approach to the study of organizational justice: A meta-analytic investigation into the consideration of normative rules, moral accountability, bandwidth-fidelity, and social exchange. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 123, 159–185. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.10.011.
Rupp, D. E., Shapiro, D. L., Folger, R., Skarlicki, D. P., & Shao, R. (2017). A critical analysis of the conceptualization and measurement of “organizational justice”: Is it time for reassessment? Academy of Management Annals, 11, 919–959. doi:10.5465/annals.2014.0051.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513.
Shapiro, D. L. (2001). The death of justice theory is likely if theorists neglect the “wheels” already invented and the voices of the injustice victims. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 235–242. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2001.1795.
Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 434–443. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.434.
Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (2004). Broadening our understanding of organizational retaliatory behavior. In R. W. Griffin & A. M. O’Leary-Kelly (Eds.), The dark side of organizational behavior (pp. 373–402). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Skarlicki, D. P., & Kulik, C. T. (2004). Third-party reactions to employee (mis)treatment: A justice perspective. Research in Organizational Behavior, 26, 183–229. doi:10.1016/S0191-3085(04)26005-1.
Skitka, L. J., Winquist, J., & Hutchinson, S. (2003). Are outcome fairness and outcome favorability distinguishable psychological constructs? A meta-analytic review. Social Justice Research, 16, 309–341.
Smith, R. W., & Kounios, J. (1996). Sudden insight: All-or-none processing revealed by speed-accuracy decomposition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 1443–1462. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1443.
Smith, C. A., & Pope, L. K. (1992). Appraisal and emotion: The interactional contributions of dispositional and situational factors. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Emotion and social behavior (Vol. 14, pp. 32–62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Spencer, S., & Rupp, D. E. (2009). Angry, guilty, and conflicted: Injustice toward coworkers heightens emotional labor through cognitive and emotional mechanisms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 429–444. doi:10.1037/a0013804.
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654. doi:10.1126/science.153.3736.652.
Thibaut, J. W., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Törnblom, K. Y., & Vermunt, R. (1999). An integrative perspective on social justice: Distributive and procedural fairness evaluations of positive and negative outcome allocations. Social Justice Research, 12, 39–64. doi:10.1023/A:1023226307252.
Van den Bos, K. (2002). Assimilation and contrast in organizational justice: The role of primed mindsets in the psychology of the fair process effect. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89, 866–880. doi:10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00033-X.
Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2001). The psychology of own versus others’ treatment: Self-oriented and other-oriented effects on perceptions of procedural justice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1324–1333. doi:10.1177/01461672012710008.
Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means of fairness judgments. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 1–60. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80003-X.
Van den Bos, K., Lind, E. A., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). How do I judge my outcome when I do not know the outcome of others? The psychology of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1034–1046. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1034.
Van den Bos, K., Maas, M., Waldring, I. E., & Semin, G. R. (2003). Toward understanding the psychology of reactions to perceived fairness: The role of affect intensity. Social Justice Research, 16, 151–168. doi:10.1023/A:1024252104717.
Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. A. M., Lind, E. A., & Vermunt, R. (1998). Evaluating outcomes by means of the fair process effect: Evidence for different processes in fairness and satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1493–1503. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1493.
Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 184–188. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01633.x.
Van Kleef, G. A. (2016). The interpersonal dynamics of emotion: Toward an integrative theory of emotions as social information. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004a). The interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 57–76. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.57.
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004b). The interpersonal effects of emotions in negotiations: A motivated information processing approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 510–528. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.510.
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2010). An interpersonal approach to emotion in social decision making: The emotions as social information model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 45–96. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42002-X.
Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C., & Cheshin, A. (2012). Emotional influence at work: Take it EASI. Organizational Psychology Review, 2, 311–339. doi:10.1177/2041386612454911.
Van Kleef, G. A., Van Doorn, E. A., Heerdink, M. W., & Koning, L. F. (2011). Emotion is for influence. European Review of Social Psychology, 22, 114–163. doi:10.1080/10463283.2011.627192.
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548.
Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Effects of justice conditions on discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 786–794. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.786.
White, H. (1988). Semantic priming of anagram solutions. American Journal of Psychology, 101, 383–399. doi:10.2307/1423086.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research. Council of Canada awarded to Annika Hillebrandt (Nos. 767-2012-1730; 766-2011-2014) and Laurie J. Barclay (No. 410-2011-1367) as well as the Ontario Early Researcher Award awarded to Laurie J. Barclay (No. ER11-08-228). We thank Amy Christie and Yujie (Jessie) Zhan for their helpful comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in our studies.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hillebrandt, A., Barclay, L.J. Observing Others’ Anger and Guilt Can Make You Feel Unfairly Treated: The Interpersonal Effects of Emotions on Justice-Related Reactions. Soc Just Res 30, 238–269 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-017-0290-5
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-017-0290-5