Social Justice Research

, Volume 22, Issue 2–3, pp 231–240 | Cite as

Conservative Shift among Liberals and Conservatives Following 9/11/01

Article

Abstract

Political orientation and political attitudes were measured in two independent adult samples. One sample was taken several months before the terrorist attacks on 9/11/01; the other, shortly after. Liberal and conservative participants alike reported more conservative attitudes following 9/11/01 than before. This conservative shift was strongest on two items with the greatest relevance to 9/11/01: George W. Bush and Increasing Military Spending. Marginally significant conservative shifts were observed on two other items (Conservatives, Socialized Medicine), and the direction of change on eight of eight items was in a conservative direction. These results provide support for the motivated social cognition model of conservatism (Jost et al., 2003) over predictions derived from terror management theory (e.g., Greenberg et al., 1992).

Keywords

Motivated social cognition Terror management theory Worldview defense Political orientation Liberals Conservatives Psychological defenses Terrorist threats 9/11 Terrorism 

References

  1. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  2. Bonanno, G. A., & Jost, J. T. (2006). Conservative shift among high-exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 311–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Echebarria-Echabe, A., & Fernández-Guede, E. (2006). Effects of terrorism on attitudes and ideological orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 259–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Gailliot, M. T., Stillman, T. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Maner, J. K., & Plant, E. A. (2008). Mortality salience increases adherence to salient norms and values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 993–1003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one’s worldview? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212–220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61, 651–670.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Jost, J. T., Fitzsimons, G., & Kay, A. C. (2004). The ideological animal: A system justification view. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 263–283). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Lambert, A., Scherer, L., Olson, K., Andrews, R., Schott, J. P., O’Brien, T., & Zisser, A. (2009). Attitude change and psychological threat: Towards a greater understanding of the role of anger vs. anxiety. Washington University, St. Louis, MO, Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  10. Landau, M., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., et al. (2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and reminders of 9/11 on support for President George W Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136–1150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McGregor, I., Nail, P. R., Marigold, D. C., & Kang, S.-J. (2005). Defensive pride and consensus: Strength in imaginary numbers after self-threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 978–996.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Nail, P. R., Harton, H. C., & Barnes, A. (2008). A test of Dovidio and Gaertner’s integrated model of racism. North American Journal of Psychology, 10, 197–220.Google Scholar
  13. Nail, P. R., Harton, H. C., & Decker, B. (2003). Political orientation and aversive versus modern racism: Tests of Dovidio and Gaertner’s integrated model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 754–770.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Nail, P. R., McGregor, I., Drinkwater, A., E., Steele, G. M., & Thompson, A. T. (in press). Threat causes liberals to think like conservatives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Google Scholar
  15. Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Sales, S. M. (1972). Economic threat as a determinant of conversion rates in authoritarian and nonauthoritarian churches. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 420–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Sales, S. M., & Friend, K. E. (1973). Success and failure as determinants of level of authoritarianism. Behavioral Science, 18, 163–172.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Thorisdottir, H., & Jost, J. (2009). Closed-mindedness mediates the effect of threat on political conservatism. Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  19. Ullrich, J., & Cohrs, C. (2007). Terrorism salience increases system justification: Experimental evidence. Social Justice Research, 20, 117–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Central ArkansasConwayUSA
  2. 2.York UniversityTorontoUSA

Personalised recommendations