Abstract
Life satisfaction, as an indicator of subjective well-being, has received increasing attention in the recent decades. It has become a potential indicator of development, to be used complementary to objective measures. However, no clear consensus exists on the relationship between life satisfaction and satisfaction with the various domains of life as well as on the measurement of life satisfaction. This paper addresses the relationship between overall life satisfaction and domain satisfaction (DS). The objective is to identify potential biases induced by priming effects when measuring DS. Four types of theoretical models, derived from existing literature, are tested in different scenarios. Data from three waves of the European Quality of Life Surveys are analyzed using a structural equation modeling framework to provide empirical evidence. An original experimental design is employed to demonstrate that priming effects cannot be ignored. A comparison of models including priming effects and those ignoring such biases shows that the former is a better fit and has higher propensity to explain the variations in life satisfaction and DS.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.

Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
For all the pure-simultaneity models that are non-recursive, the stability indexes are <1. With some caution (see Kline 2006, 59), this means that the estimates can be analyzed as such. When not including priming effects, minimization is unsuccessful, indicating the extremely poor fit of the model.
References
Althaus, S. L., & Kim, Y. M. (2006). Priming effects in complex information environments: Reassessing the impact of news discourse on presidential approval. Journal of Politics, 68(4), 960–976.
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social Indicators of well-being. Americans’ perceptions of life quality. New York: Plenum Press.
Biemer, P. P., & Lyberg, L. E. (2003). Introduction to survey quality. New York: Wiley.
Böhnke, P. (2005). First European quality of life survey: Life satisfaction, happiness and sense of belonging. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities.
Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 646–653.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The quality of American life perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Casas, F., Figuer, C., González, M., & Coenders, G. (2004). Satisfaction with life domains and salient values for future: Data from children and their parents in five different countries. In W. Glatzer, S. von Below, & M. Stoffregen (Eds.), Challenges for the quality of life in contemporary societies (pp. 233–248). Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Cesario, J. (2014). Priming, replication, and the hardest science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 40–48.
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464–504.
Christoph, B., & Noll, H. (2003). Subjective well-being in the European Union during the 90s. Social Indicators Research, 64(3), 521–546.
Comşa, M. (2010). How to compare means of latent variables across countries and waves: Testing for invariance measurement. An application using eastern European societies. Sociológia, 42(6), 639–669.
Converse, J. M., & Presser, S. (1986). Survey questions: Handcrafting the standardized questionnaire. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Costa, P. T., & McRae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.
Crano, W. D., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Principles and methods of social research. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cummins, R. A. (2003). Normative life satisfaction: Measurement issues and a homeostatic model. Social Indicators Research, 64(2), 225–256.
Cummins, R. A., Lau, A. L. D., & Davern, M. T. (2012). Subjective wellbeing homeostasis. In K. C. Land, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of social indicators and quality of life research (pp. 79–98). Berlin: Springer.
Delhey, J. (2004). Life satisfaction in an enlarged Europe. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.
Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157.
Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1997). Measuring quality of life: Economic, social, and subjective indicators. Social Indicators Research, 40, 189–216.
Domke, D., Shah, D. V., & Wackman, D. B. (1998). Media priming effects: Accessibility, association, and activation. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 10(1), 51–74.
Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G, Jr. (1988). Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement of belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 421–435.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.
Harris, J. L., Bargh, J. A., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior. Health Psychology, 28(4), 404.
Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 81–100.
Hsieh, C. M. (2008). The relative importance of health. Social Indicators Research, 84(1), 127–137.
Hutchison, K. A., et al. (2013). The semantic priming project. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1099–1114.
Kaplan, S. A., Luchman, J. N., & Mock, L. (2013). General and specific question sequence effects in satisfaction surveys: Integrating directional and correlational effects. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(5), 1443–1458.
Kim, S.-H., Han, M., & Scheufele, D. A. (2010). Think about him this way: Priming, news media, and South Koreans’ evaluation of the president. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 22(3), 299–319.
Kline, R. B. (2006). Reversed arrows dynamics. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 43–68). Charlotte, NC: IAP.
Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1987). An evaluation of a cognitive theory of response order effects in survey measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 201–219.
Lance, C. E., Lautenschlager, G. J., Sloan, C. E., & Varca, P. E. (1989). A comparison between bottom-up, top-down, and bidirectional models of relationships between global and life facet satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 57(3), 601–624.
Leonardi, F., Spazzafumo, L., & Marcellini, F. (2005). Subjective well-being: The constructionist point of view. A longitudinal study to verify the predictive power of top-down effects and bottom-up processes. Social Indicators Research, 70, 53–77.
Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies theory (MDT). In A. C. Michalos (Ed.), Citation Classics from social indicators research. The most cited articles edited and introduced by Alex C. Michalos (pp. 305–372). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Mihalache, F. F. (2008). Top-down vs. bottom-up: Two perspectives in explaining life satisfaction. Calitatea vieţii, 1–2, 71–83.
Ortells, J. J., Vellido, C., Daza, M. T., & Noguera, C. (2007). Semantic priming effects with and without perceptual awareness. Psicológica, 27(2), 225–242.
Rode, J. C. (2004). Job satisfaction and life satisfaction revisited: A longitudinal test of an integrated model. Human Relations, 57(9), 1205–1230.
Rode, J. C., & Near, J. P. (2005). Spillover between work attitude and overall life attitudes: Myth or reality? Social Indicators Research, 70(1), 79–100.
Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70, 345–384.
Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys: Experiments on question form, wording, and context. New York: Academic Press.
Suls, J., & Wheeler, L. (2012). Social comparison. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 460–482). London: Sage Publications.
Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying context effects in attitude measurement. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 299–314.
Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Veenhoven, R. (2007). Subjective measures of well-being. In McGillivray (Ed.), Human well-being. Concept and measurement, houndmills, New Hampshire (pp. 214–239). USA: Palgrave/McMillan.
Voicu, B., & Pop, C. (2011). Measurement models of life satisfaction: A SEM approach. Calitatea Vieţii, 2(2011), 137–154.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the CNCS-UEFISCDI grants PN-II-ID-PCE-2011-3-0210 and PN-II-ID-PCE-2011-3-0669. The author thanks Paula Tufiş, Marian Vasile and Mircea Comşa for valuable comments on early versions of the paper.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Voicu, B. Priming Effects in Measuring Life Satisfaction. Soc Indic Res 124, 993–1013 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-014-0818-0
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-014-0818-0