Skip to main content
Log in

Priming Effects in Measuring Life Satisfaction

Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Cite this article

Abstract

Life satisfaction, as an indicator of subjective well-being, has received increasing attention in the recent decades. It has become a potential indicator of development, to be used complementary to objective measures. However, no clear consensus exists on the relationship between life satisfaction and satisfaction with the various domains of life as well as on the measurement of life satisfaction. This paper addresses the relationship between overall life satisfaction and domain satisfaction (DS). The objective is to identify potential biases induced by priming effects when measuring DS. Four types of theoretical models, derived from existing literature, are tested in different scenarios. Data from three waves of the European Quality of Life Surveys are analyzed using a structural equation modeling framework to provide empirical evidence. An original experimental design is employed to demonstrate that priming effects cannot be ignored. A comparison of models including priming effects and those ignoring such biases shows that the former is a better fit and has higher propensity to explain the variations in life satisfaction and DS.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For all the pure-simultaneity models that are non-recursive, the stability indexes are <1. With some caution (see Kline 2006, 59), this means that the estimates can be analyzed as such. When not including priming effects, minimization is unsuccessful, indicating the extremely poor fit of the model.

References

  • Althaus, S. L., & Kim, Y. M. (2006). Priming effects in complex information environments: Reassessing the impact of news discourse on presidential approval. Journal of Politics, 68(4), 960–976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social Indicators of well-being. Americans’ perceptions of life quality. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Biemer, P. P., & Lyberg, L. E. (2003). Introduction to survey quality. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Böhnke, P. (2005). First European quality of life survey: Life satisfaction, happiness and sense of belonging. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 646–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The quality of American life perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casas, F., Figuer, C., González, M., & Coenders, G. (2004). Satisfaction with life domains and salient values for future: Data from children and their parents in five different countries. In W. Glatzer, S. von Below, & M. Stoffregen (Eds.), Challenges for the quality of life in contemporary societies (pp. 233–248). Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cesario, J. (2014). Priming, replication, and the hardest science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 40–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoph, B., & Noll, H. (2003). Subjective well-being in the European Union during the 90s. Social Indicators Research, 64(3), 521–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comşa, M. (2010). How to compare means of latent variables across countries and waves: Testing for invariance measurement. An application using eastern European societies. Sociológia, 42(6), 639–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, J. M., & Presser, S. (1986). Survey questions: Handcrafting the standardized questionnaire. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McRae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crano, W. D., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Principles and methods of social research. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. A. (2003). Normative life satisfaction: Measurement issues and a homeostatic model. Social Indicators Research, 64(2), 225–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. A., Lau, A. L. D., & Davern, M. T. (2012). Subjective wellbeing homeostasis. In K. C. Land, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of social indicators and quality of life research (pp. 79–98). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Delhey, J. (2004). Life satisfaction in an enlarged Europe. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1997). Measuring quality of life: Economic, social, and subjective indicators. Social Indicators Research, 40, 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domke, D., Shah, D. V., & Wackman, D. B. (1998). Media priming effects: Accessibility, association, and activation. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 10(1), 51–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G, Jr. (1988). Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement of belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 421–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. L., Bargh, J. A., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior. Health Psychology, 28(4), 404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 81–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, C. M. (2008). The relative importance of health. Social Indicators Research, 84(1), 127–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison, K. A., et al. (2013). The semantic priming project. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1099–1114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S. A., Luchman, J. N., & Mock, L. (2013). General and specific question sequence effects in satisfaction surveys: Integrating directional and correlational effects. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(5), 1443–1458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S.-H., Han, M., & Scheufele, D. A. (2010). Think about him this way: Priming, news media, and South Koreans’ evaluation of the president. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 22(3), 299–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2006). Reversed arrows dynamics. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 43–68). Charlotte, NC: IAP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1987). An evaluation of a cognitive theory of response order effects in survey measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lance, C. E., Lautenschlager, G. J., Sloan, C. E., & Varca, P. E. (1989). A comparison between bottom-up, top-down, and bidirectional models of relationships between global and life facet satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 57(3), 601–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonardi, F., Spazzafumo, L., & Marcellini, F. (2005). Subjective well-being: The constructionist point of view. A longitudinal study to verify the predictive power of top-down effects and bottom-up processes. Social Indicators Research, 70, 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies theory (MDT). In A. C. Michalos (Ed.), Citation Classics from social indicators research. The most cited articles edited and introduced by Alex C. Michalos (pp. 305–372). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mihalache, F. F. (2008). Top-down vs. bottom-up: Two perspectives in explaining life satisfaction. Calitatea vieţii, 1–2, 71–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortells, J. J., Vellido, C., Daza, M. T., & Noguera, C. (2007). Semantic priming effects with and without perceptual awareness. Psicológica, 27(2), 225–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rode, J. C. (2004). Job satisfaction and life satisfaction revisited: A longitudinal test of an integrated model. Human Relations, 57(9), 1205–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rode, J. C., & Near, J. P. (2005). Spillover between work attitude and overall life attitudes: Myth or reality? Social Indicators Research, 70(1), 79–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70, 345–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys: Experiments on question form, wording, and context. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., & Wheeler, L. (2012). Social comparison. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 460–482). London: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying context effects in attitude measurement. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 299–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2007). Subjective measures of well-being. In McGillivray (Ed.), Human well-being. Concept and measurement, houndmills, New Hampshire (pp. 214–239). USA: Palgrave/McMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voicu, B., & Pop, C. (2011). Measurement models of life satisfaction: A SEM approach. Calitatea Vieţii, 2(2011), 137–154.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the CNCS-UEFISCDI grants PN-II-ID-PCE-2011-3-0210 and PN-II-ID-PCE-2011-3-0669. The author thanks Paula Tufiş, Marian Vasile and Mircea Comşa for valuable comments on early versions of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bogdan Voicu.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Voicu, B. Priming Effects in Measuring Life Satisfaction. Soc Indic Res 124, 993–1013 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-014-0818-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-014-0818-0

Keywords

Navigation