Hedonic Versus Eudaimonic Conceptions of Well-being: Evidence of Differential Associations With Self-reported Well-being

Abstract

Conceptions of well-being are cognitive representations of the nature and experience of well-being. These conceptions can be described generally by the degree to which hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions are emphasized as important aspects of the experience of well-being. In two studies, the prediction that eudaimonic dimensions of individual conceptions of well-being are more robustly associated with self-reported well-being than hedonic dimensions was investigated. Correlational analyses indicated that both hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions were associated with well-being, with more robust associations observed between the eudaimonic dimension and each measure of well-being. In several regression analyses, only the eudaimonic dimension significantly predicted well-being, with the hedonic dimension failing to account for unique variance in well-being beyond that predicted by the eudaimonic dimension. Results thus generally suggest that conceptualizing well-being in eudaimonic terms may be relatively more important for positive psychological functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Notes

  1. 1.

    The most appropriate way for researchers to conceptualize well-being is a topic that is still under debate (e.g., Kashdan et al. 2008), and consequently a standard operationalization of well-being does not yet exist. We argue that well-being includes both hedonic and eudaimonic components and, accordingly, operationalize well-being using measures of both hedonic (e.g., subjective well-being) and eudaimonic (e.g., meaning in life) aspects of well-being.

  2. 2.

    One may assume that the current results have implications concerning the validity of eudaimonic and hedonic philosophies in general. Discourse concerning the validity of hedonic and eudaimonic approaches has a long history within moral philosophy and psychology (see Ryan and Deci 2001), and a full discussion of this topic is beyond the scope of this article. However, we emphasize that the validity of any particular philosophy is not necessarily based on the degree to which it is associated with the experience of well-being. Thus, although the findings of the current study indicate that eudaimonic aspects of individual conceptions of well-being are more robustly associated with several self-report indices of well-being than are hedonic aspects, we do not assume that these findings necessarily indicate that a eudaimonic philosophy is more valid than a hedonic philosophy.

References

  1. Arbuckle, J. L. (2009). Amos 18.0. Crawfordville, FL: Amos Development Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Aristotle. (2000). Nicomachean ethics. R. Crisp (Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  3. Bauer, J. J., McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. J. (2008). Narrative identity and eudaimonic well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 81–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Belliotti, R. A. (2004). Happiness is overrated. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Birnbaum, M. H. (2000). Psychological experiments on the internet. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the internet. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 803–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Apley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New York, NY: Academic Press.

  9. Brunstein, J. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Carrol, E. N., & Zuckerman, M. (1977). Psychopathology and sensation seeking in ‘downers’, ‘speeders’, and ‘trippers’: A study of the relationship between personality and drug choice. International Journal of the Addictions, 12, 591–601.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larson, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 130–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself: An inquiry into the psychology of ethics. New York: Fawcett.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation modelling with the SIMPLIS command language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Kagan, S. (1992). The limits of well-being. In E. F. Paul, F. D. Miller Jr., & J. Paul (Eds.), The good life and the human good (pp. 169–189). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kashdan, T. B., Biswas-Diener, R., & King, L. A. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: The costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 219–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. King, L. A., & Napa, C. K. (1998). What makes life good? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 156–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Larson, R. J. (2003). Measuring positive emotions. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Positive psychological assessment: A handbook of models and measures (pp. 201–218). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Martin, M. W. (2008). Paradoxes of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 171–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Massimini, F., & Delle Fave, A. (2000). Individual development in a bio-cultural perspective. American Psychologist, 55, 24–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. McMahan, E. A., & Estes, D. (2010). Measuring lay conceptions of well-being: The beliefs about well-being scale. Journal of Happiness Studies. Published online. doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9194-x.

  32. Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Holleran, S. E., & Clark, C. S. (2010). Eavesdropping on happiness: Well-being is related to having less small talk and more substantive conversations. Psychological Science, 21, 539–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Mill, J. S. (1989). Autobiography. New York, NY: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Ng, A. K., Ho, D. Y. F., Wong, S. S., & Smith, I. (2003). In search of the good life: A cultural odyssey in the East and West. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 129, 317–363.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2001). Pleasures and subjective well-being. European Journal of Personality, 15, 153–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Park, N., Peterson, C., & Ruch, W. (2009). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction in twenty-seven nations. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 273–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 25–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Pflug, J. (2009). Folk theories of happiness: A cross-cultural comparison of conceptions of happiness in Germany and South Africa. Social Indicators Research, 92, 551–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. M. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 719–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: A eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 13–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Shweder, R. A. (1998). Welcome to middle age! (and other cultural fictions). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Sidgwick, H. (1907). Methods of ethics (7th ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Steger, M. F. (2009). Meaning in life. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (pp. 679–687). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 80–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good by doing good: Daily eudaimonic activity and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 22–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Tseng, W. C. (2007). A study of folk concepts of the good life and subjective well-being among college students. Bulletin of Educational Psychology, 38, 417–441.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Waterman, A. S. (2005). When effort is enjoyed: Two studies of intrinsic motivation for personally salient activities. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 165–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. S., & Conti, R. (2008). The implications of two conceptions of happiness (hedonic enjoyment and eudaimonia) for the understanding of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 41–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 59, 339–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Zuckerman, M. (2009). Sensation seeking. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences and social behavior (pp. 455–465). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ethan A. McMahan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McMahan, E.A., Estes, D. Hedonic Versus Eudaimonic Conceptions of Well-being: Evidence of Differential Associations With Self-reported Well-being. Soc Indic Res 103, 93–108 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9698-0

Download citation

Keywords

  • Well-being
  • Lay conceptions
  • Hedonism
  • Eudaimonia
  • Pleasure
  • Happiness