How Preschoolers Associate Power with Gender in Male-Female Interactions: A Cross-Cultural Investigation

Abstract

Interactions between males and females often display a power imbalance. Men tend to adopt more dominant physical postures, lead conversations more, and are more likely to impose their will on women than vice versa. Furthermore, social representations typically associate males with a higher power than females. However, little is known about how those representations emerge in early childhood. The present study investigated whether preschool children from different countries assign more power to males than to females in the context of mixed-gender interactions. In Experiments 1a (n = 148) and 1b (n = 403), which implemented power through body postures, 4–6 year-old children from France, Lebanon, and Norway strongly associated power with a male character. Experiment 2 (n = 160) showed that although both French boys and girls identified themselves more with a dominant than with a subordinate posture, girls were less likely to do so in a mixed-gender context. In Experiment 3 (n = 213), which no longer used body postures, boys from Lebanon and France attributed more decision power and resource control to a male puppet than did girls. By investigating gender representations through interactions, the present study shows that children associate gender and power at an early age.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1

References

  1. Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/0471249688.

  2. Albert, A. A., & Porter, J. R. (1986). Children’s gender role stereotypes: A comparison of the United States and South Africa. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17(1), 45–65. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002186017001004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Anderson, K. J., & Leaper, C. (1998). Meta-analyses of gender effects on conversational interruption: Who, what, when, where, and how. Sex Roles, 39(3–4), 225–252. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018802521676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Barkow, J. H. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology, 16(4), 553–563. https://www.jstor.org/stable/2741630.

  5. Baxter, J., & Kane, E. W. (1995). Dependence and independence: A cross-national analysis of gender inequality and gender attitudes. Gender & Society, 9(2), 193–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/089124395009002004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.88.4.354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bernard, S., Castelain, T., Mercier, H., Kaufmann, L., Van der Henst, J.-B., & Clément, F. (2016). The boss is always right: Preschoolers endorse the testimony of a dominant over that of a subordinate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 307–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Best, D. L., Williams, J. E., Cloud, J. M., Davis, S. W., Robertson, L. S., Edwards, J. R., … Fowles, J. (1977). Development of sex-trait stereotypes among young children in the United States, England, and Ireland. Child Development, 48, 1375–1384. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1977.tb03943.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bianchi, S. M., Milkie, M. A., Sayer, L. C., & Robinson, J. P. (2000). Is anyone doing the housework? Trends in the gender division of household labor. Social Forces, 79(1), 191–228. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/79.1.191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Bird, R. (1999). Cooperation and conflict: The behavioral ecology of the sexual division of labor. Evolutionary Anthropology, 8(2), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-6505(1999)8:2%3C65::aid-evan5%3E3.3.co;2-v.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Birnbaum, D. W., & Chemelski, B. E. (1984). Preschoolers’ inferences about gender and emotion: The mediation of emotionality stereotypes. Sex Roles, 10(7–8), 505–511.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Birnbaum, D. W., Nosanchuk, T. A., & Croll, W. L. (1980). Children’s stereotypes about sex differences in emotionality. Sex Roles, 6(3), 435–443. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Boseovski, J. J. (2010). Evidence for “rose-colored glasses”: An examination of the positivity bias in young children’s personality judgments. Child Development Perspectives, 4(3), 212–218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00149.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Brey, E., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children use nonverbal cues to make inferences about social power. Child Development, 86(1), 276–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Brightman, R. (1996). The sexual division of foraging labor: Biology, taboo, and gender politics. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 38(4), 687–729. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0010417500020508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106(4), 676–713. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Carli, L. L. (1999). Gender, interpersonal power, and social influence. Journal of Social Issues, 55(1), 81–99. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Cashdan, E. (1998). Smiles, speech, and body posture: How women and men display sociometric status and power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(4), 209–228. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022967721884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J. B., & Mercier, H. (2016). The influence of power and reason on young Maya children’s endorsement of testimony. Developmental Science, 19(6), 957–966. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Kaufman, L., Clement, F., Berchtold, A., Reboul, A., … Van Der Henst, J.-B. (2015). How preschoolers use cues of dominance to make sense of their social environment. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(4), 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.926269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clément, F., Kaufmann, L., Reboul, A., & Van Der Henst, J.-B. (2016). Children’s allocation of resources in social dominance situations. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1843–1857. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Charafeddine, R., Yamada, Y., Matsui, T., Sudo, M., Germain, P., Bernard, S., … Van Der Henst, J.-B. (2019). Cross-cultural differences in the valuing of dominance by young children. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 19(3–4), 256–272. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Charlesworth, W. R., & Dzur, C. (1987). Gender comparisons of preschoolers’ behavior and resource utilization in group problem solving. Child Development, 58(1), 191–200. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Clément, F., Harris, P., Bernard, S., Antonietti, J.-P., & Kaufmann, L. (2014). Rousseau’s child. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 73(2), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Cogsdill, E. J., Todorov, A. T., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2014). Inferring character from faces. Psychological Science, 25(5), 1132–1139. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614523297.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Cowan, G., & Hoffman, C. D. (1986). Gender stereotyping in young children: Evidence to support a concept-learning approach. Sex Roles, 14(3–4), 211–224. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00288250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66(3), 710–722. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt aggression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 579–588. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cunningham, S. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). The colour of gender stereotyping. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 598–614. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02023.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Darmangeat, C. (2009). Le communisme primitif n’est plus ce qu’il était : Aux origines de l’oppression des femmes [primitive communism is no longer what it was: The origins of women’s oppression]. Toulouse: Smolny.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Dovidio, J. F., Ellyson, S. L., Keating, C. F., Heltman, K., & Brown, C. E. (1988). The relationship of social power to visual displays of dominance between men and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 233–242. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2016). The development of implicit gender attitudes. Developmental Science, 19(5), 781–789. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 3–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0090375.

  34. Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1998). Are women less selfish than men?: Evidence from dictator experiments. The Economic Journal, 108(448), 726–735. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0297.00311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Edelman, M. S., & Omark, D. R. (1973). Dominance hierarchies in young children. Social Science Information, 12(1), 103–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/053901847301200105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (1980). Differential socialization of girls and boys in light of cross-cultural research. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1980(8), 45–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219800807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Enright, E. A., Gweon, H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2017). ‘To the victor go the spoils’: Infants expect resources to align with dominance structures. Cognition, 164, 8–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Esposito, A. (1979). Sex differences in children’s conversation. Language and Speech, 22(3), 213–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097902200302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children’s play qualities in same-, other-, and mixed-sex peer groups. Child Development, 74(3), 921–932. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M. D., & O’boyle, C. (1992). Gender labeling, gender stereotyping, and parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 28(2), 225–230. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Fagot, B. I., Rodgers, C. S., & Leinbach, M. D. (2012). Theories of gender socialization. In T. Eckes & H. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 79–104). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9781410605245/chapters/10.4324/9781410605245-10.

  42. Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454(7208), 1079–1083. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Fishbein, H. D., & Imai, S. (1993). Preschoolers select playmates on the basis of gender and race. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 14(3), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(93)90012-k.

  44. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Gazes, R. P., Hampton, R. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2017). Transitive inference of social dominance by human infants. Developmental Science, 20(2), e12367. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Giles, J. W., & Heyman, G. D. (2005). Young children’s beliefs about the relationship between gender and aggressive behavior. Child Development, 76(1), 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00833.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gneezy, U., Leonard, K. L., & List, J. A. (2009). Gender differences in competition: Evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society. Econometrica, 77(5), 1637–1664. https://doi.org/10.3386/w13727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Goffman, E. (1977). The arrangement between the sexes. Theory and Society, 4(3), 301–331. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00206983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Gong, B., Yan, H., & Yang, C.-L. (2015). Gender differences in the dictator experiment: Evidence from the matrilineal Mosuo and the patriarchal Yi. Experimental Economics, 18(2), 302–313. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1655689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Guinote, A., Cotzia, I., Sandhu, S., & Siwa, P. (2015). Social status modulates prosocial behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(3), 731–736. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414550112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Gülgöz, S., & Gelman, S. A. (2017). Who’s the boss? Concepts of social power across development. Child Development, 88(3), 946–963. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Halim, M. L., Ruble, D. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2013). Four-year-olds' beliefs about how others regard males and females. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(1), 128–135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02084.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 898–924. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp. 505-570). New Jersey: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0309

  56. Harter, S., & Pike, R. (1984). The pictorial scale of perceived competence and social acceptance for young children. Child Development, 55(6), 1969–1982. https://doi.org/10.1037/t04621-000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 19(1), 97–132. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1998.0470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Hayden-Thomson, L., Rubin, K. H., & Hymel, S. (1987). Sex preferences in sociometric choices. Developmental Psychology, 23(4), 558–562. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.4.558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Henley, N. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex, and nonverbal communication. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. https://doi.org/10.2307/2065224.

  60. Henley, N. M., & Harmon, S. (1985). The nonverbal semantics of power and gender: A perceptual study. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 151–164). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5106-4_8.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Hilliard, L. J., & Liben, L. S. (2010). Differing levels of gender salience in preschool classrooms: Effects on children’s gender attitudes and intergroup bias. Child Development, 81(6), 1787–1798. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01510.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Horne, R. M., Johnson, M. D., Galambos, N. L., & Krahn, H. J. (2018). Time, money, or gender? Predictors of the division of household labour across life stages. Sex Roles, 78(11), 731–743. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-017-0832-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Human Rights Watch. (2015). Unequal and unprotected: Women’s rights under Lebanon’s religious personal status laws. https://doi.org/10.1163/2210-7975_hrd-2156-2015061.

  64. Keating, C. F., & Bai, D. L. (1986). Children’s attributions of social dominance from facial cues. Child Development, 57(5), 1269–1276. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1986.tb00454.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 581–592. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Kuhn, D., Nash, S. C., & Brucken, L. (1978). Sex role concepts of two- and three-year-olds. Child Development, 49(2), 445–451. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. La Freniere, P., Strayer, F. F., & Gauthier, R. (1984). The emergence of same-sex affiliative preferences among preschool peers: A developmental/ethological perspective. Child Development, 55(5), 1958–1965. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00437.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Lakoff, R. (1973). Language and woman’s place. Language in Society, 2(1), 45–79. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0047404500000051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Lammers, J., & Stapel, D. A. (2011). Power increases dehumanization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14(1), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210370042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Lane, I. M., & Coon, R. C. (1972). Reward allocation in preschool children. Child Development, 43(4), 1382–1389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1972.tb02092.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12(1), 107–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885-2014(97)90032-0.

  72. Levy, G. D., Taylor, M. G., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). Traditional and evaluative aspects of flexibility in gender roles, social conventions, moral rules, and physical laws. Child Development, 66(2), 515–531. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00887.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2001). Pink and blue collar jobs: Children’s judgments of job status and job aspirations in relation to sex of worker. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(4), 346–363. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2011). Pretty in pink: The early development of gender-stereotyped colour preferences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 656–667. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02027.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. LoBue, V., Nishida, T., Chiong, C., DeLoache, J. S., & Haidt, J. (2011). When getting something good is bad: Even three-year-olds react to inequality. Social Development, 20(1), 154–170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00560.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Lourenco, S. F., Bonny, J. W., & Schwartz, B. L. (2016). Children and adults use physical size and numerical alliances in third-party judgments of dominance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2050–2060. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02050.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Lukaszewski, A. W., Simmons, Z. L., Anderson, C., & Roney, J. R. (2016). The role of physical formidability in human social status allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 385–406. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Ma, L., & Woolley, J. D. (2013). Young children’s sensitivity to speaker gender when learning from others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(1), 100–119. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.638687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 20, 239–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(08)60404-8.

  80. Mandalaywala, T. M., Rhodes, M., & Tai, C. (2019). Children's use of race and gender as cues to social status. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3rnej.

  81. Mansour, Z., & Karam, S. (2012). School-related gender based violence (SRGBV) in Lebanon. Beirut. UNESCO Regional Bureau for Education in the Arab States. https://unesdoc.unesco.org/ark:/48223/pf0000219623.

  82. Martin, C. L., & Halverson Jr., C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52(4), 1119–1134. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903–933. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Martin, C. L., Andrews, N. C., England, D. E., Zosuls, K., & Ruble, D. N. (2017). A dual identity approach for conceptualizing and measuring children's gender identity. Child Development, 88(1), 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(18), 6862–6867. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113194109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Factors in the division of labor by sex: A cross-cultural analysis. Ethnology, 12(2), 203–225. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Neff, K. D., Cooper, C. E., & Woodruff, A. L. (2007). Children’s and adolescents’ developing perceptions of gender inequality. Social Development, 16(4), 682–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00411.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Nelson, A. (2005). Children’s toy collections in Sweden—A less gender-typed country? Sex Roles, 52(1–2), 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-1196-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Olson, K. R., Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., & Weisman, K. G. (2012). Children associate racial groups with wealth: Evidence from South Africa. Child Development, 83(6), 1884–1899. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01819.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Omark, D. R., & Edelman, M. S. (1975). A comparison of status hierarchies in young children: An ethological approach. Social Science Information, 14(5), 87–107. https://doi.org/10.1177/053901847501400505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Omark, D. R., Strayer, F. F., & Freedman, D. G. (1980). Dominance relations: An ethological view of human conflict and social interaction. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Ostrov, J. M., & Keating, C. F. (2004). Gender differences in preschool aggression during free play and structured interactions: An observational study. Social Development, 13(2), 255–277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.000266.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2015). Children infer affiliative and status relations from watching others imitate. Developmental Science, 18(6), 917–925. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Parker, R., Wellings, K., & Lazarus, J. V. (2009). Sexuality education in Europe: An overview of current policies. Sex Education, 9(3), 227–242. https://doi.org/10.1080/14681810903059060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Picariello, M. L., Greenberg, D. N., & Pillemer, D. B. (1990). Children’s sex-related stereotyping of colors. Child Development, 61(5), 1453–1460. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Pickert, S. M., & Wall, S. M. (1981). An investigation of children’s perceptions of dominance relations. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52(1), 75–81. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1981.52.1.75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Pun, A., Birch, S. A., & Baron, A. S. (2016). Infants use relative numerical group size to infer social dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(9), 2376–2381. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1514879113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Ridgeway, C. L. (2013). Gender, interaction, and inequality. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Rochat, P., Dias, M. D., Liping, G., Broesch, T., Passos-Ferreira, C., Winning, A., … Berg, B. (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3-and 5-year-olds across seven cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(3), 416–442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022109332844

  102. Rosaldo, M. Z., Lamphere, L., & Bamberger, J. (1974). Woman, culture, and society. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  103. Rossholt, N. (2003). Barnehagen og førskolelærerutdanningen i et kjønns-, likestillings- og diskursperspektiv [Kindergartens and early childhood education in a gender, equality and discourse perspective]. Oslo: Høgskolen i Oslo.

    Google Scholar 

  104. Schmid-Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145–165. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:jonb.0000039647.94190.21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Sebanc, A. M., Pierce, S. L., Cheatham, C. L., & Gunnar, M. R. (2003). Gendered social worlds in preschool: Dominance, peer acceptance and assertive social skills in boys’ and girls’ peer groups. Social Development, 12(1), 91–106. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Selten, R., & Ockenfels, A. (1998). An experimental solidarity game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 34(4), 517–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(97)00107-8.

  107. Shaw, A., & Olson, K. R. (2012). Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 382–395. https://doi.org/10.1037/e519402009-001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental Science, 13(4), 599–610. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00913.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Shutts, K., Kenward, B., Falk, H., Ivegran, A., & Fawcett, C. (2017). Early preschool environments and gender: Effects of gender pedagogy in Sweden. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/8wj2p.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Sluckin, A. M., & Smith, P. K. (1977). Two approaches to the concept of dominance in preschool children. Child Development, 48(3), 917–923. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Stipek, D., & Iver, D. M. (1989). Developmental change in children’s assessment of intellectual competence. Child Development, 60(3), 521–538. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb02734.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Strayer, F. F., Chapeskie, T. R., & Strayer, J. (1978). The perception of preschool social dominance. Aggressive Behavior, 4(2), 183–192. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2337(1978)4:2%3C183::aid-ab2480040209%3E3.0.co;2-g.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Tabet, P. (1979). Les Mains, les outils, les armes [Hands, Tools, Weapons]. L’Homme, 19(3/4), 5–61. https://doi.org/10.3406/hom.1979.367998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), Organizational identity: A reader (pp. 56–65). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  115. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Taylor, M. G., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Boys will be boys; cows will be cows: Children’s essentialist reasoning about gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 80(2), 461–481. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01272.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Terrier, N., Bernard, S., Mercier, H., & Clément, F. (2016). Visual access trumps gender in 3- and 4-year-old children’s endorsement of testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 223–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Terrizzi, B. F., Brey, E., Shutts, K., & Beier, J. S. (2018). Children’s developing judgments about the physical manifestations of power. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 793–808. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/z75nu.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Terrizzi, B. F., Woodward, A. M., & Beier, J. S. (2019). Young children and adults associate social power with indifference to others’ needs. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/45uca.

  121. Thomas, A. J., Thomsen, L., Lukowski, A. F., Abramyan, M., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2018). Toddlers prefer those who win but not when they win by force. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 662–669. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0415-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., & Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: Preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance. Science, 331(6016), 477–480. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 519–545. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Toscano, H., Schubert, T. W., & Sell, A. N. (2014). Judgments of dominance from the face track physical strength. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Turner, P. J., Gervai, J., & Hinde, R. A. (1993). Gender-typing in young children: Preferences, behaviour and cultural differences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 323–342. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1993.tb00607.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Weisfeld, G. E., & Beresford, J. M. (1982). Erectness of posture as an indicator of dominance or success in humans. Motivation and Emotion, 6(2), 113–131. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00992459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Weisgram, E. S., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2010). Gender, values, and occupational interests among children, adolescents, and adults. Child Development, 81(3), 778–796. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01433.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  128. Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. M. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children’s toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 401–409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender & Society, 1(2), 125–151 10.1177%2F0891243287001002002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Whyte, M. K. (1978). The status of women in preindustrial societies. Princeton: Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400871810-001.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1982). Measuring sex stereotypes. A thirty-nation study. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  132. Williams, J. E., Bennett, S. M., & Best, D. L. (1975). Awareness and expression of sex stereotypes in young children. Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 635–642. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.11.5.635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Yavorsky, J. E., Dush, C. M. K., & Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. (2015). The production of inequality: The gender division of labor across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77(3), 662–679. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Yee, M., & Brown, R. (1994). The development of gender differentiation in young children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01017.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Zammuner, V. L. (1982). Sex role stereotypes in Italian children. International Journal of Psychology, 17(1–4), 43–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207598208247431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Rawan Charafeddine Laboratoire Langage Cerveau Cognition, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod; Imac Maria Zambrana, Faculty of Educational Science, Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo; Benoît Triniol, Laboratoire Langage Cerveau Cognition, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod; Hugo Mercier, Laboratoire Langage Cerveau Cognition, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod; Fabrice Clément, Centre de Sciences Cognitives, Université de Neuchâtel; Laurence Kaufmann, Institut des Sciences Sociales, Faculté des Sciences Sociales et Politiques, Lausanne University; Anne Reboul, Laboratoire Langage Cerveau Cognition, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod; Francisco Pons, Faculty of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo; Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst, Laboratoire Langage Cerveau Cognition, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod

We wish to thank Emily Hewitt, Bérangère Testud, Mayssan Charafeddine, Léonore Ferrer Catala and Antoine Danielou for help with data collection. We also thank the participating children and their parents, the schools and Inspection Académique de Lyon, in particular Sylvie Coustier, Stéphane Garapon, Vincent Guili and Michèle Prieur, for making this research possible. We thank Gloria Origgi for her valuable input; Justine Epinat, Thomas Castelain, Audrey Breton, Thomas Charavet-Gomel and Ira Noveck for their support and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. This research was supported by a grant from Fondation de France and by CNRS (Défi Genre) awarded to the last author.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rawan Charafeddine.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOCX 37 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Charafeddine, R., Zambrana, I.M., Triniol, B. et al. How Preschoolers Associate Power with Gender in Male-Female Interactions: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. Sex Roles 83, 453–473 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01116-x

Download citation

Keywords

  • Power
  • Gender
  • Dominance
  • Preschoolers
  • Mixed-gender interactions
  • Cross-cultural comparisons