Shifts in Women’s Mate Preferences Across the Ovulatory Cycle: A Critique of Harris (2011) and Harris (2012)

Abstract

Many studies have documented systematic shifts in women’s mate preferences and sexual motivations across the ovulatory cycle. Harris (2012) presents a nonreplication of one particular finding in this literature—namely, that women’s preference for masculinity in men’s faces shifts across the cycle. Harris critiques the empirical and theoretical literature on cycle shifts and concludes that the cycle shift hypothesis should be abandoned. Here, we situate Harris’s findings within the broader empirical literature and respond to several of the points in her critique. We conclude that the evidence for cycle shifts in women‘s mate preferences and sexual motivations is much stronger than Harris portrays and that she mischaracterizes the theoretical basis of this work.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References

  1. Anderson, K. G. (2006). How well does paternity confidence match actual paternity? Evidence from worldwide nonpaternity rates. Current Anthropology, 47, 435–461. doi:10.1086/504167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Boothroyd, L. G., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., DeBruine, L. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 211–218. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Boothroyd, L. G., Cross, C. P., Gray, A. W., Coombes, C., & Gregson-Curtis, K. (2011). Perceiving the facial correlates of sociosexuality: Further evidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 422–425. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance to violence: Mate retention tactics in married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 346–361. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.72.2.346.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  5. DeBruine, L., Jones, B. C., Frederick, D. A., Haselton, M. G., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Evidence for menstrual cycle shifts in women’s preferences for masculinity. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 768–775.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., . . . Perrett, D. I. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 215–222. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004

  7. Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19, 82–87. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20583.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Flowe, H. D., Swords, E., & Rockey, J. C. (2012). Women’s behavioural engagement with a masculine male heightens during the fertile window: Evidence for the cycle shift hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 285–290. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Folstad, I., & Karter, A. J. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. American Naturalist, 139, 603–622. doi:10.1086/285346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Frederick, D. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2007). Why is muscularity sexy? Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1167–1183. doi:10.1177/0146167207303022.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Furuichi, T., & Hashimoto, C. (2002). Why female bonobos have a lower copulation rate during estrus than chimpanzees. In C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, & L. Marchant (Eds.), Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos (pp. 156–167). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511606397.016.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preference for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 265, 927–933. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2008). Human oestrus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 991–100. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in women’s sexual interests and their partners’ mate-retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 975–982. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203–207. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Women’s sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle depend on primary partner developmental instability. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 2023–2027. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gangestad, S. W., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Simpson, J. A., & Cousins, A. J. (2007). Changes in women’s mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 151–163. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.151.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010a). Fertility in the cycle predicts women’s interest in sexual opportunism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 400–411. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010b). Men’s facial masculinity predicts changes in their female partners’ sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle, whereas men’s intelligence does not. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 412–424. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gangestad, S. W., Merriman, L. A., & Emery Thompson, M. (2010). Men’s oxidative stress, fluctuating asymmetry and physical attractiveness. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1005–1013. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., Miller, R. D., & Olp, J. J. (2006). Major Histocompatibility Complex alleles, sexual responsivity, and unfaithfulness in romantic couples. Psychological Science, 17, 830–835. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.007.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2008). Hormonal correlates of women’s mid-cycle preference for the scent of symmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 223–232. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Gilbert, S. F. (2000). Developmental biology. Sunderland: Sinauer. doi:10.1006/dbio.1996.0032.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. (2012). Do womens mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Paper presented at the conference of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, Albuquerque, NM.

  25. Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. (2013). Do womens mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Poster presented at the conference of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, New Orleans, LA.

  26. Hale, G. E., Zhao, X., Hughes, C. L., Burger, H. G., Robertson, D. M., & Fraser, I. S. (2007). Endocrine features of menstrual cycles in middle and late reproductive age and the menopausal transition classified according to the staging of reproductive aging (STRAW) staging system. The Journal of Clinical Endocriniology & Metabolism, 92, 3060–3067. doi:10.1210/jc.2007-0066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Harris, C. R. (2011). Menstrual cycle and facial preferences reconsidered. Sex Roles, 64, 669–681. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9772-8.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Harris, C. R. (2012). Shifts in masculinity preferences across the menstrual cycle: Still not there. Sex Roles. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0229-0.

  29. Haselton, M. G. (2012). Fertile minds: Changes in human social behavior across the ovulatory cycle. Paper presented at the conference of the Association for Psychological Science, Chicago, IL.

  30. Haselton, M. G., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Conditional expression of women’s desires and men’s mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 509–518. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.10.006.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Haselton, M. G., & Gangestad, S. W. (2012). Human estrus and pair-bonding. Talk presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Annual Conference, Albuquerque, New Mexico.

  32. Haselton, M. G., & Gildersleeve, K. (2011). Can men detect ovulation? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 87–92. doi:10.1177/0963721411402668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Jones, B.C., Perrett, D.I., Little, A.C., Boothroyd, L.G., Cornwell, R.E., Feinberg, D.R., . . . Moore, F.R. (2005). Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 347-354. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2962

  34. Larson, C. M., Pillsworth, E. G., & Haselton, M. G. (2012). Ovulatory shifts in women’s attractions to primary partners and other men: Further evidence of the importance of primary partner sexual attractiveness. PLoS One, 7, e44456. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044456.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Variation in facial masculinity and symmetry preferences across the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 999–10008. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & Burriss, R. P. (2007). Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 633–639. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 76, 209–216. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.003.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Mikolajczyk, R. T., & Stanford, J. B. (2005). A new method for estimating the effectiveness of emergency contraception that accounts for variation in timing of ovulation and previous cycle length. Fertility and Sterility, 83, 1764–1770. doi:10.1016/j.fertnstert.2005.01.097.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Millar, M. (2013). Menstrual cycle changes in mate preferences for cues associated with genetic quality: The moderating role of mate value. Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 18–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 39–48. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00033-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., et al. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741–742. doi:10.1038/21557.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Perrett, D. I., Lee, K., Penton-Voak, I. S., Rowland, D. A., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., . . . Akamatsu, S. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887. doi:10.1038/29772

    Google Scholar 

  43. Pillsworth, E. G., & Haselton, M. G. (2006). Male sexual attractiveness predicts differential ovulatory shifts in female extra-pair attraction and male mate retention. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 247–258. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 388–397. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Puts, D. A. (2006). Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits: Potential hormonal causes. Human Nature, 17, 114–127. doi:10.1007/s12110-006-1023-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Rantala, M. J., Coetzee, V., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Kecko, S., Krama, T., . . . Krams, I. (2013). Adiposity, compared with masculinity, serves as a more valid cue to immunocompetence in human mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280(1751). doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.2495

  47. Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, S93–S95. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 186–201. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Rhodes, G., Morley, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2013). Women can judge sexual unfaithfulness from unfamiliar men’s faces. Biology Letters, 9(1). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.0908.

  50. Scelza, B. A. (2011). Female choice and extra-pair paternity in a traditional human population. Biology Letters, 7, 889–891. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0478.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Scott, I. M. L., Clark, A. P., Boothroyd, L. G. & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Do men’s faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioral Ecology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1093/beheco/ars092

  52. Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 575–584. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1177.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Stumpf, R. M., & Boesch, C. (2005). Does promiscuous mating preclude female choice? Female sexual strategies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of the Tai National Park, Cote d’lvoire. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, 511–524. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0868-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 131–144. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The evolutionary biology of human female sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Watts, D. P. (1991). Mountain gorilla reproduction and sexual behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 24, 211–225. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350240307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Wegienka, G., & Day Baird, D. (2005). A comparison of recalled date of last menstrual period with prospectively recorded dates. Journal of Women's Health, 14, 248–252. doi:10.1089/jwh.2005.14.248.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wood, W., Kressel, L., Joshi, P., & Louie, B. (2012). Women’s mate preferences: A meta-analysis of menstrual cycle effects. Manuscript Submitted for Publication.

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa DeBruine.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gildersleeve, K., DeBruine, L., Haselton, M.G. et al. Shifts in Women’s Mate Preferences Across the Ovulatory Cycle: A Critique of Harris (2011) and Harris (2012). Sex Roles 69, 516–524 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0273-4

Download citation

Keywords

  • Menstrual cycle
  • Ovulatory cycle
  • Ovulation
  • Facial masculinity
  • Mate preferences
  • Sexual motivations