Advertisement

Sex Roles

, 65:712 | Cite as

A League of Their Own: Do Single-Sex Schools Increase Girls’ Participation in the Physical Sciences?

  • Isabelle D. CherneyEmail author
  • Kaitlin L. Campbell
Original Article

Abstract

With the rapid shifts in the education of women in the United States, and the underrepresentation of women in fields of science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM), an issue generating much controversy is whether women may benefit more from single-sex education or coeducation. The present study surveyed 548 U.S. high-school boys and girls from single-sex and coeducational high-schools from the Midwest. Half of the participants completed a mathematics test under stereotype threat (ST) condition and half under no threat condition. Although girls in single-sex schools had higher achievement motive and self-esteem than those in coeducational schools, they were not more likely to pursue STEM careers. Overall, students in single-sex schools outperformed students from coeducational schools on the math test. Girls’ math performance was significantly higher in the ST condition than in the no threat condition.

Keywords

Single-sex schools Coeducational schools Gender differences Stereotype threat Occupational choices Adolescents 

Notes

Acknowledgement

This study was in part supported by a summer grant from Creighton University’s Graduate School to the first author. We thank Samuel Pierre, Kunal Sualy, Kristin Jones, Maren Hankey, Katerina Anastasiou, Samantha Lewis, Samantha Brown, Hannah Grawe, Jack Kostal, and Nicolas Villanueva for their assistance with data collection and coding. We also thank the various high-schools for their participation. Preliminary data were presented at the Gender Development Conference in San Francisco, 2010, and at the International Congress of Psychology in Berlin, 2008.

References

  1. Adams, J., Priest, R. F., & Prince, H. T., II. (1985). Achievement motive: Analyzing the validity of the WOFO. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 9, 357–370. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altermatt, E. R., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2003). The development of competence-related and motivational beliefs: An investigation of similarity and influence among friends. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 111–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00291.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The work preference inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 950–967. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.950.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29–46. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
  6. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition, (Vol. 10 pp. 1–6). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Belcher, C., Frey, A., & Yankeelov, P. (2006). The effects of single-sex classrooms on classroom environment, self-esteem, and standardized test scores. School Social Work Journal, 31, 61–75.Google Scholar
  8. Ben-Zeev, T., Duncan, S., & Forbes, C. (2005a). Stereotypes and math performance. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 235–249). New York: Psychology.Google Scholar
  9. Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (2005b). Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown, R. P., & Josephs, R. A. (1999). A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76, 246–257. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (Eds.). (2007). Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/11546-000.Google Scholar
  12. Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218–261. doi: 10.1037/a001/4412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chouinard, R., Vezeau, C., & Bouffard, T. (2008). Coeducational or single-sex school: Does it make a difference on high-school academic motivation? Educational Studies, 34, 129–144. doi: 10.1080/03055690701811180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Collaer, M. L., & Nelson, J. D. (2002). Large visuospatial sex difference in line judgment: Possible role of attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 41, 1–12. doi: 10.1006/breg.2001.1321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Of men, women, and motivation: A role congruity account. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 434–447). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  17. Diekman, A. B., Brown, E. R., Johnston, A. M., & Clark, E. K. (2010). Seeking congruity between goals and roles: A new look at why women opt out of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics careers. Psychological Science, 21, 1051–1057. doi: 10.1177/0956797610377342.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  19. Eccles, J. S. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 283–331). Greenwich: JAI.Google Scholar
  20. Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women’s educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585–609. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb01049.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. doi: 10.1O37//OO22-3514.80.3.501.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Farmer, H. S., Wardrop, J. L., & Rotella, S. C. (1999). Antecedent factors differentiating women and men in science/nonscience careers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23, 763–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 1–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x.Google Scholar
  24. Harker, R. (2000). Achievement, gender, and the single-sex/coed debate. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 21, 203–218. doi: 10.1080/713655349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hattie, J. (2002). Classroom composition and peer effects. International Journal of Educational Research, 37, 449–481. doi: 10.1016/S0883-0355(03)00015-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 545–560. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in math performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychological Science, 11, 365–371. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2003). Do high-achieving female students underperform in private? The implications of threatening environments on intellectual processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 796–805. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Keller, J. (2007). Stereotype threat in classroom settings: The interactive effect of domain identification, task difficulty and stereotype threat on female students’ math performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 323–338. doi: 10.1348/000709906X113662.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kessels, U., & Hannover, B. (2008). When being a girl matters less: Accessibility of gender-related self-knowledge in single-sex and mixed classes and its impact on students’ physics-related self-concept of ability. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 273–289. doi: 10.1348/000709907X215938.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kray, L. J., Thompson, L., & Galinsky, A. D. (2001). Battle of the sexes: Gender stereotype confirmation and reactance in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Personality, 80, 942–958. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kurdek, L. A., & Sinclair, R. J. (2000). Psychological, family, and peer predictors of academic outcomes in first- through fifth-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 449–457. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lawrie, L., & Brown, R. (1992). Sex stereotypes, school subject preferences and career aspirations as a function of single/mixed-sex schooling and presence/absence of an opposite sex sibling. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 132–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lee, V. E., & Bryk, A. S. (1986). Effects of single-sex secondary schools on student achievement and attitudes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 381–395. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.78.5.381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lepper, M. R. (1988). Motivational considerations in the study of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 5, 289–309. doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci0504_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 184–196. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lubinski, D. S., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Sex differences in personal attributes for the development of scientific expertise. In S. J. Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t there more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 79–100). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/11546-007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 520–540. doi: 10.2307/749772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mael, F. A., Alonso, A. D., Rogers, K., & Smith, M. (2005). Single-sex versus coeducational schooling: A systematic review. Washington, DC: US Department of Education.Google Scholar
  41. Marini, M. M., & Greenberger, E. (1978). Sex differences in educational aspirations and expectations. American Educational Research Journal, 15, 67–79. doi: 10.2307/1162688.Google Scholar
  42. Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women’s math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1183–1193. doi: 10.1177/01461672022812004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Monaco, N. M., & Gaier, E. L. (1992). Single-sex versus coeducational environment and achievement in adolescent females. Adolescence, 27, 579–594.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. National Science Foundation. (2008, Feburary). Science and Engineering indicators. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind08/?govDel=USNSF_124
  46. O’Brian, E. (1985). Global self-esteem scales: Unidimensional or multidimensional? Psychological Reports, 57, 383–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Quinn, D. N., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women’s generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 55–72. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Riordan, C. (1990). Girls and boys in school: Together or separate? New York: Teachers College.Google Scholar
  49. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development, (5th ed., pp. 610–700). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  51. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Journal, 25, 54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Ryan, K. E., & Ryan, A. M. (2005). Psychological processes underlying stereotype threat and standardized math test performance. Educational Psychologist, 40, 53–63. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4001_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). “I don’t feel like it”: The function of interest in self-regulation. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self regulation (pp. 203–228). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  54. Schoon, I. (2001). Teenage job aspirations and career attainment in adulthood: A 17-year follow-up study of teenagers who aspired to become scientists, health professionals, or engineers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 124–132. doi: 10.1080/01650250042000186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80–83. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Snyder, T. D., Dillow, S. A., & Hoffman, C. M. (2009). Digest of education statistics, 2008 (NCES 2009-020). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences.Google Scholar
  57. Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  58. Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1983). Achievement-related motives and behavior. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  59. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: A meta-analysis of sex difference in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 859–884. doi: 10.1037/a0017364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tauer, J. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). The effects of cooperation and competition on intrinsic motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 849–861. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.849.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tidball, M. (1973). Perspective on academic women and affirmative action. Educational Record, 54, 130–135.Google Scholar
  64. Watson, C., Quatman, T., & Edler, E. (2002). Career aspirations of adolescent girls: Effects of achievement level, grade, and single-sex school environment. Sex Roles, 46, 323–335. doi: 10.1023/A:1020228613796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation: Metaphors, theories, and research. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  66. Wiegfield, A., Battle, A., Keller, L. B., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Sex differences in motivation, self-concept, career aspiration, and career choice: Implications for cognitive development. In A. V. McGillicuddy-De Lisi & R. De Lisi (Eds.), Biology, society, and behavior: The development of sex differences in cognition (pp. 93–124). Westport: Greenwood.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCreighton UniversityOmahaUSA

Personalised recommendations