Abstract
Eagly’s social role theory (Eagly and Steffen 1984) was tested examining children’s gender role stereotypes via implicit information processing and memory measures. We explored whether children’s occupational stereotypes were less restrictive for females who engaged in counterstereotypic occupations (Mary-Doctor) compared to males who engaged in counterstereotypic occupations (Henry-Nurse). Fifty-seven American eight- and nine-year-olds from a southwestern city were orally presented with stereotypic male and female names paired with masculine and feminine occupations and asked to create sentences using the name-occupation pairs. We conducted analyses of the created sentences as well as tested children’s memories for the various pairings. Consistent with social role theory, the findings revealed that children’s gender role stereotypes were more restrictive for males, than for females.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.



Similar content being viewed by others
References
Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136–141.
Barry, H., III, & Harper, A. S. (1995). Increased choice of female phonetic attributes in first names. Sex Roles, 32, 809–819.
Bauer, P. (1993). Memory for gender-consistent and gender-inconsistent event sequences by twenty-five-month-old children. Child Development, 64, 285–297.
Bauer, P., & Coyne, M. (1997). When the name says it all: Preschoolers’ recognition and use of the gendered nature of common proper names. Social Development, 6, 271–291.
Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. Signs, 8, 598–616.
Bennett, M., Sani, F., Hopkins, N., Agostini, L., & Malucchi, L. (2000). Children’s gender categorization: An investigation of automatic processing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 97–102.
Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., & Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London-B, 271, 1143–1450.
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1990). The role of attitudes and intervention in gender-schematic processing. Child Development, 61, 1440–1452.
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1992). Cognitive mechanisms in children’s gender stereotyping: Theoretical and educational implications of a cognitive-based intervention. Child Development, 63, 1351–1363.
Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1142–1163.
Blakemore, J. E. W. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys. Sex Roles, 48, 411–419.
Blau, F. D., Ferber, M. A., & Winkler, A. E. (2002). The economics of women, men, and work. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.
Cann, A. (1993). Evaluative expectations and the gender schema: Is failed inconsistency better? Sex Roles, 28, 667–678.
Carter, D. B., & McCloskey, L. A. (1983–1984). Peers and the maintenance of sex-typed behavior: The development of children’s conceptions of cross-gender behavior in their peers. Social Cognition, 2, 294–314.
Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the gender segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413–423.
Conkright, L., Flannagan, D., & Dykes, J. (2000). Effects of pronoun type and gender role consistency on children’s recall and interpretations of stories. Gender Roles, 43, 481–198.
Correll, S. J. (2004). Constraints into preferences: Gender, status and emerging career aspirations. American Sociological Review, 69, 93–113.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18.
Devine, P. G. (2001). Implicit prejudice and stereotyping: How automatic are they? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 757–868.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Woman and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188.
Diekman, A. B., & Goodfriend, W. (2006). Rolling with the changes: A role congruity perspective on gender norms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 369–383.
Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2003). The malleability of sex differences in response to changing social roles. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology (pp. 103–115). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735–754.
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of gender differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eccles, J. S., Barber, D. M., & Jozefowicz, D. L. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational, and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois, & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.), Genderism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence (pp. 153–198). Washington, DC: APA.
Fagot, B. I. (1977). Consequences of moderate cross-gender behavior in preschool children. Child Development, 48, 902–907.
Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1993). Gender-role development in egalitarian and traditional families. Sex Roles, 32, 513–526.
Fischer, S. C., Hickey, D. T., Pellegrino, J. W., & Law, D. J. (1994). Strategic processing in dynamic spatial tasks. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 65–105.
Frawley, T. J. (2008). Gender schema and prejudicial recall: How children misremember, fabricate, and distort gendered picture book information. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 22, 291–303.
Garrett, C. S., Ein, P. L., & Tremaine, P. L. (1977). The development of gender stereotyping of adult occupations in elementary school children. Child Development, 48, 507–512.
Halpern, D. F. (2000). Gender differences in cognitive abilities. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Hayghe, H. (1990). Family members in the work force. Monthly Labor Review, 113, 14–19.
Helwig, A. A. (1998). Gender-role stereotyping: Testing theory with a longitudinal sample. Sex Roles, 38, 403–423.
Hochschild, A. R. (1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home. New York: Viking.
Hughes, F. M., & Seta, C. E. (2003). Gender stereotypes: Children’s perceptions of future compensatory behavior following violations of gender roles. Sex Roles, 49, 685–691.
Kail, R. V., & Levine, L. E. (1976). Encoding processes and sex-role preferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 256–263.
Kee, D. W., & Guttentag, R. (1994). Resource requirements of knowledge access and recall: Benefits of associative strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 211–223.
Kimmel, M. S. (2004). The Gendered Society. New York: Oxford University Press.
Koblinsky, S. G., Cruse, D. F., & Sugawara, A. I. (1978). Sex role stereotypes and children’s memory for story content. Child Development, 452–458.
Levy, G. D., Taylor, M. G., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). Traditional and evaluative aspects of flexibility in gender roles, social conventions, moral rules, and physical laws. Child Development, 66, 515–531.
Levy, G. D., Sadovsky, A. L., & Troseth, G. L. (2000). Aspects of young children’s perceptions of gender-typed occupations. Sex Roles, 42, 993–1006.
Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. L. (1980). Gender-related schemata and constructive memory in children. Child Development, 51, 11–18.
Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. L. (1993). Gender-schematic processing in children: The role of initial interpretations of stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 29, 141–149.
Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2001). Pink and blue collar jobs: Children’s judgments of job status and job aspirations in relation to gender of worker. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 346–363.
Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2002). Language at work: Children’s gendered interpretations of occupational titles. Child Development, 73, 810–828.
Macrae, C. N., Mitchell, J. P., & Pendry, L. F. (2002). What’s in a forename? Cue familiarity and stereotypical thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 186–193.
Martin, C. L. (1993). New directions for investigating children’s gender knowledge. Developmental Review, 13, 184–204.
Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of gender typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.
Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1983). The effects of gender-typing schemas on young children’s memory. Child Development, 54, 1119–1134.
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.
Most, S. B., Sorber, A. V., & Cunningham, J. G. (2007). Auditory Stroop reveals implicit gender associations in adults and children. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 287–294.
Nadelman, L. (1974). Sex identity in American children: Memory, knowledge, and preference tests. Developmental Psychology, 10, 413–417.
Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., & Wasserman, W. (1996). Applied linear statistical models. Boston: McGraw Hill College.
O’Brien, M., Peyton, V., Mistry, R., Hruda, L., Jacobs, A., Cladera, Y., et al. (2000). Gender role cognition in three-year old boys and girls. Sex Roles, 42, 10071026.
Perez, S. M., & Kee, D. W. (2000). Girls not boys show gender-connotation encoding from print. Sex Roles, 42, 439–447.
Ridgeway, C. L., & Correll, S. J. (2004). Unpacking the gender system: A theoretical perspective on gender beliefs and social relations. Gender and Society, 18, 510–531.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr., Rabinowitz, M., & Dronkers, N. F. (1982). Event knowledge, elaborative propensity, and the development of learning proficiency. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 492–503.
Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 858–932). Hoboken: Wiley.
Sherman, J. W., Stroessner, S. J., Loftus, S. T., & Deguzman, G. (1997). Stereotype suppression and recognition memory for stereotypical and nonstereotypical information. Social Cognition, 15, 205–215.
Signorella, M. L., & Liben, L. S. (1985). Assessing children’s gender-stereotyped attitudes. Psychological Documents, 15, 7. Ms. No. 2685.
Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children’s gender schema about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13, 147–183.
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological Review, 96, 58–83.
Stangor, C. (1988). Stereotype accessibility and information processing. Personality and Social Bulletin, 14, 694–708.
Stangor, C., & McMillian, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42–61.
Stangor, C., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Stereotype development and memory: What we remember depends on how much we know. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 18–35.
Stoddard, T., & Turiel, E. (1985). Children’s conceptions of cross-gender activities. Child Development, 56, 1241–1252.
Trice, A. D., & Rush, K. (1995). Gender-stereotyping in four-year-olds’ occupational aspirations. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 701–702.
United States Department of Labor (1996, March). The 20 leading occupations of employed women in 1996. Retrieved from http:/www.dol.gov/dol/wb/public/wb_pubs/20lead96.htm.
United States Department of Labor (1999, March). Nontraditional occupations for women in 1999. Retrieved from http://www.dol.gov/dol/ wb/public/wb_pubs/nontra99.htm.
Acknowledgement
We wish to express our sincerest appreciation of Christy Ake, Allie Gregory, Julie Ann Hwang, Kyrstle Barrera, Jui Bhagwat, Paula Daneri, and Alana Bennett for data coding. In addition, we would like to sincerely thank Cate Taylor, Jui Bhagwat, Vrinda Kalia, Stephanie Martinez, and Tianlun (Tony) Jiang for their suggestions on previous drafts of the manuscript. We are also greatly indebted to Margaret Signorella as well as an anonymous reviewer for their invaluable and extensive comments and suggestions that greatly improved the manuscript. Finally, we would also like to acknowledge Michele Johnson and the third-grade students that participated in the study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendix
Appendix
Means (+ Standard Deviations) on Dependent Measures as a function of Participant’s Gender
Boys (n = 25) | Girls (n = 32) | |||||||
Dependent Measure | Mm | Mf | Ff | Fm | Mm | Mf | Ff | Fm |
Sentence Response Latencya (in seconds) | 2.39 (1.70) | 2.88 (2.54) | 2.48 (1.55) | 2.36 (1.71) | 2.95 (2.19) | 3.48 (2.39) | 2.83 (1.46) | 2.71 (1.96) |
Sentence Omissions (%) | 2.40 (6.63) | 11.20 (16.41) | 7.20 (12.75) | 10.40 (17.44) | 4.38 (11.05) | 13.13 (13.06) | 10.00 (13.44) | 8.12 (15.12) |
Unincorporated Sentences (%) | 7.61 (12.68) | 12.62 (19.85) | 9.22 (14.12) | 5.21 (8.72) | 12.21 (18.62) | 17.70 (17.13) | 11.62 (17.23) | 14.08 (17.57) |
Recall Correct (%) | 12.80 (15.14) | 8.00 (12.91) | 3.20 (7.48) | 5.60 (10.83) | 10.00 (11.36) | 11.25 (16.80) | 6.89 (14.01) | 5.00 (10.16) |
Number of Recall Errorsb | Male Names | Female Names | Male Names | Female Names | ||||
Stereotype Congruent | 3.0 (1.47) | 2.72 (1.43) | 2.66 (1.41) | 2.94 (1.59) | ||||
Stereotype Incongruent | 1.92 (1.32) | 2.28 (1.54) | 1.28 (1.05) | 1.44 (1.41) |
Examples of Occupation Incorporated and Occupation Unincorporated Created Sentences.
Occupation Incorporated
Andrew the police officer arrested a bad guy.
Steve the janitor cleans the classrooms.
Brad the librarian works in the Library.
David the housekeeper cleans the house.
Julie the schoolteacher teaches math.
Patricia the nurse helps sick people.
Susan the auto mechanic fixes cars.
Heather the firefighter puts out fires
Occupation Unincorporated
William the doctor is a doctor.
Henry the nurse is a man.
Mark the secretary is a principal.
Heather the telephone operator is a girl.
Julie the schoolteacher is funny.
Patricia the janitor is a pretty janitor.
Kathy the doctor is woman who is a doctor.
Henry the nurse is a doctor too.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wilbourn, M.P., Kee, D.W. Henry the Nurse is a Doctor Too: Implicitly Examining Children’s Gender Stereotypes for Male and Female Occupational Roles. Sex Roles 62, 670–683 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9773-7
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9773-7