Skip to main content
Log in

How Sexy are Sexist Men? Women’s Perception of Male Response Profiles in the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In Studies 1 to 3, German female students (total N = 326) rated the likability and typicality of male targets: a nonsexist, a benevolent sexist, a hostile sexist, and (in Studies 2 and 3) an ambivalent sexist. When targets were presented as response profiles in the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (Glick and Fiske 1996) (Studies 2 and 3), the benevolent sexist was rated to be most likable but least typical, whereas the ambivalent sexist was rated to be highly typical. Thus, women were aware of a link between benevolent and hostile sexism and approved of men’s benevolent sexism, especially when it was not paired with hostile sexism. Likability ratings were moderated by participants’ own benevolent sexism and feminist attitude.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams, D., Viki, G. T. N., Masser, B., & Bohner, G. (2003). Perceptions of stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent and hostile sexism in victim blame and rape proclivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 111–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: How it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 633–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branscombe, N. R., & Deaux, K. (1991). Feminist attitude accessibility and behavioral intentions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 411–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary pespective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B. (1993). Influence: science and practice (3rd ed.). New York: Harpercollins College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dardenne, B., Dumont, M., & Bollier, T. (2007). Insidious dangers of benevolent sexism: Consequences for women’s performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 764–779.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ditto, P. H., & Jemmott, J. B. I. I. I. (1989). From rarity to evaluative extremity: Effects of prevalence information on evaluations of positive and negative characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 16–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckes, T. (2001). Ambivalenter Sexismus und die Polarisierung von Geschlechterstereotypen [Ambivalent sexism and the polarization of gender stereotypes]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 32, 235–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckes, T., & Six-Materna, I. (1999). Hostilität und Benevolenz: Eine Skala zur Erfassung des ambivalenten Sexismus [Hostility and benevolence: A scale for the assessment of ambivalent sexism]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 30, 211–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (1995). Ambivalence and stereotypes cause sexual harassment: A theory with implications for organizational change. Journal of Social Issues, 51(1), 97–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B., et al. (2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 763–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences for specific and diffuse forms of system justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 498–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kilianski, S. E., & Rudman, L. A. (1998). Wanting it both ways: Do women approve of benevolent sexism? Sex Roles, 39, 333–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short-term mates: What, whether, and why. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 468–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, B. L. (1996). Putting the feminism into feminism scales: Introduction of a liberal feminist attitude and ideology scale (LFAIS). Sex Roles, 34, 359–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moya, M., Glick, P., Expósito, F., de Lemus, S., & Hart, J. (2007). It’s for your own good: Benevolent sexism and women’s reactions to protectively justified restrictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1421–1434.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siebler, F., Sabelus, S., & Bohner, G. (2008). A refined computer harassment paradigm: Validation, and test of hypotheses about target characteristics. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 22–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K., Mallett, R., Russo-Devosa, Y., & Stangor, C. (2005). Judgments of sexism: A comparison of the subtlety of sexism measurs and sources of variability in judgments of sexism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 406–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations (2008). Human development report 2007/2008. Retrieved January 5, 2009, from the United Nations Development Programme Web site: http://hdr.undp.org/en/media/HDR_20072008_EN_Complete.pdf.

  • Viki, G. T., Abrams, D., & Hutchison, P. (2003). The "true" romantic: Benevolent sexism and paternalistic chivalry. Sex Roles, 49, 533–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Preliminary reports of this research were given as invited presentations at Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile (September 2004), the University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany (June 2006), the University of Basel, Switzerland (April 2007), the University of Magdeburg, Germany (May 2007), the Universities of Cape Town and Pretoria, South Africa (August 2007), and the University of Graz, Austria (March 2008). We thank Caroline Erdmann for her help with the data collection and analyses in Study 1, and the "Astoria" cinema, Bielefeld, for providing free tickets as incentives for participants in Study 2. Thanks also to Friederike Eyssel and Nina Vanselow, who provided helpful comments on a previous draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerd Bohner.

Appendix

Appendix

German Items Used to Assess Participants’ Benevolent Sexism and Hostile Sexism and to Construct Male Sexism Profiles

Numbers correspond to item numbers in Glick and Fiske (1996). No item numbers were presented to participants.

Abbreviations in parentheses pertain to Studies 2 and 3: PA=item was used in version A of profiles; PB=item was used in version B of profiles; QA=item was used in version A of participant questionnaire; QB=item was used in version B of participant questionnaire.

  1. 1.

    Egal wie erfolgreich ein Mann auch sein mag, ohne eine Frau, die ihn liebt, fehlt ihm etwas sehr Wichtiges. (PA, QB)

  2. 2.

    Viele Frauen versuchen unter dem Deckmantel der Gleichberechtigung, besondere Vergünstigungen zu erlangen, z. B. eine Bevorzugung bei der Besetzung der Arbeitsstelle. (PB, QA)

  3. 3.

    Bei einer Katastrophe sollten Frauen vor Männern gerettet werden. (PB, QA)

  4. 4.

    Die meisten Frauen interpretieren harmlose Äußerungen oder Handlungen als frauenfeindlich. (PA, QB)

  5. 5.

    Frauen sind zu schnell beleidigt. (PA, QB)

  6. 6.

    Man kann in seinem Leben erst richtig glücklich sein, wenn man einen Partner hat, den man liebt. [Modified in Studies 1 and 3 to enhance clarity of meaning: Ein Mann kann in seinem Leben erst richtig glücklich sein, wenn er eine Partnerin hat, die er liebt.] (PA, QB)

  7. 7.

    Was Feministinnen wirklich wollen, ist, dass Frauen mehr Macht bekommen als Männer. (PB, QA)

  8. 8.

    Viele Frauen besitzen eine Art von Ehrlichkeit, die nur wenige Männer besitzen. (QA, QB)

  9. 9.

    Frauen sollten von Männern umsorgt und beschützt werden. (PA, QB)

  10. 10.

    Die meisten Frauen sehen gar nicht, was Männer alles für sie tun. (PB, QA)

  11. 11.

    Frauen versuchen, Macht zu erlangen, indem sie Männer immer mehr beherrschen. (QA, QB)

  12. 12.

    Jeder Mann sollte eine Frau haben, die er wirklich liebt. (PA, QB)

  13. 13.

    Männer sind ohne Frauen unvollkommen. (PB, QA)

  14. 14.

    Frauen übertreiben Probleme, die sie am Arbeitsplatz haben. (PA, QB)

  15. 15.

    Hat eine Frau erst mal einen Mann ”rumgekriegt“, dann versucht sie, ihn an die kurze Leine zu legen. (PB, QA)

  16. 16.

    Wenn Frauen in einem fairen Wettkampf gegenüber Männern den Kürzeren ziehen, behaupten sie gerne, sie seien diskriminiert worden. (PA, QB)

  17. 17.

    Eine Frau sollte von ihrem Mann auf Händen getragen werden. (PA, QB)

  18. 18.

    Viele Frauen haben Spaß daran, mit Männern zu “spielen“, indem sie sich erst verführerisch geben, und dann aber die Annäherungsversuche der Männer zurückweisen. (PB, QA)

  19. 19.

    Verglichen mit Männern haben Frauen das bessere moralische Empfinden. (PB, QA)

  20. 20.

    Ein Mann sollte bereit sein, sein eigenes Wohl zu opfern, um für seine Frau zu sorgen. (PB, QA)

  21. 21.

    Feministinnen stellen an Männer vollkommen überzogene Forderungen. (PA, QB)

  22. 22.

    Verglichen mit Männern haben Frauen einen feineren Sinn für Kultur und einen besseren Geschmack. (PA, QB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bohner, G., Ahlborn, K. & Steiner, R. How Sexy are Sexist Men? Women’s Perception of Male Response Profiles in the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory. Sex Roles 62, 568–582 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9665-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9665-x

Keywords

Navigation