Abstract
Researchers interested in the associations of gender with spatial experience and spatial ability have not yet focused on several activities that have become common in the modern digital age. In this study, using a new questionnaire called the Survey of Spatial Representation and Activities (SSRA), we examined spatial experiences with computers and videogames in a sample of nearly 1,300 undergraduate students. Large gender differences, which favored men, were found in computer experience. Although men and women also differed on SAT scores, gender differences in computer experience were still apparent with SAT factored out. Furthermore, men and women with high and low levels of computer experience, who were selected for more intensive study, were found to differ significantly on the Mental Rotations Test (MRT). Path analyses showed that computer experience substantially mediates the gender difference in spatial ability observed on the MRT. These results collectively suggest that the “Digital Divide” is an important phenomenon and that encouraging women and girls to gain spatial experiences, such as computer usage, might help to bridge the gap in spatial ability between the sexes.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.Similar content being viewed by others
References
Akhter, S. (2003). Digital divide and purchase intention: Why demographic psychology matters. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 321–327.
Alington, D., Russell, L., & Monaghan, J. (1992). Effects of stimulus color, pattern, and practice on sex differences in mental rotations task performance. Journal of Psychology, 126, 539–553.
Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–344.
Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1995). Environmental input to the development of sex-related differences in spatial and mathematical ability. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 363–379.
Barron, B. (2004). Learning ecologies for the technological fluency: Gender and experience differences. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 31, 1–36.
Casey, M., Colon, D., & Goris, Y. (1992). Family handedness as a predictor of mental rotation ability among minority girls in a math-science training program. Brain and Cognition, 18, 88–96.
Cooper, J. (2003). Gender and computers: Understanding the digital divide. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
De Lisi, R., & Cammarano, D. (1996). Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 12, 351–361.
De Lisi, R., & Wolford, J. (2002). Improving children's mental rotation accuracy with computer game playing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163, 272–283.
Dominick, J. (1984). Videogames, television violence, and aggression in teenagers. Communication, 34, 136–147.
Dorval, M., & Pepin, M. (1986). Effect of playing a video game on a measure of spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 159–162.
Drori, G., & Jang, Y. (2003). The global digital divide: A sociological assessment of trends and causes. Social Science Computer Review, 21, 144–161.
Duesbury, R., & O'Neil, H. (1996). Effect of type of practice in a compare-aided design environment in visualizing three-dimensional objects from two-dimensional orthographic projections. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 249–260.
Gagnon, D. (1985). Videogames and spatial skills: An exploratory study. Educational Communication and Technology, 33, 263–275.
Global Internet Trends. (2001, First Quarter). 420 million people with internet access. Retrieved August, 2003, from http://www.matrade.gov.my/ecommerce/news-archive/stat-4htm.
Goodman, L. (1960). On the exact variance of products. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 55, 708–713.
Greenfield, P., Brannon, G., & Lohr, D. (1994). Two-dimensional representation of movement through three-dimensional space: The role of videogame expertise. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 87–103.
Grimshaw, G., Sitarenios, G., & Finegan, J. (1995). Mental rotation at 7 years: Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition, 29, 85–100.
Hess, R., & Niura, I. (1985). Gender differences in enrollment in computer camps and classes. Sex Roles, 13, 193–203.
James, K., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Beliefs about self and about gender groups: Interactive effects on the spatial performance of women. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 411–425.
Joseph, J., & Willingham, D. (2000). Effect of sex and joystick experience on pursuit tracking in adults. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 45–56.
Klatzky, R., Golledge, R., Loomis, J., Cicinelli, J., & Pellegrino, J. (1995). Performance of blind and sighted persons on spatial tasks. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 89, 70–82.
Larson, P., Rizzo, A., Buckwalter, J., Van Rooyen, A., Kratz, K., Neumann, U., et al. (1999). Gender issues in the use of virtual environments. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 2, 113–123.
Lawton, C. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30, 765–779.
Lawton, C., & Morrin, K. (1999). Gender differences in pointing accuracy in computer-simulated 3D mazes. Sex Roles, 40, 73–93.
Linn, M., & Petersen, A. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.
Livingstone, S. (2003). Children's use of the internet: Reflections on the emerging research agenda. New Media and Society, 5, 147–166.
MacKinnon, D., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. (1995). A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30, 41–62.
Masters, M. (1998). The gender difference on the Mental Rotation Test is not due to performance factors. Memory and Cognition, 26, 444–448.
McClurg, P., & Chaille, C. (1987). Computer games: Environments for developing spatial cognition? Journal of Educational Computing Research, 3, 95–111.
Moody, M. (1997). Changes in scores on the Mental Rotation Test during the menstrual cycle. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 955–961.
Morlock, L., & Yamanaka, E. (1985). Measuring women's attitudes, goals, and literacy toward computers and advanced technology. Educational Technology, 25, 12–14.
Nordvik, H., & Amponsah, B. (1998). Gender differences in spatial abilities and spatial activity among university students in an egalitarian educational system. Sex Roles, 38, 1009–1023.
Okagaki, L., & Frensch, P. (1994). Effects of videogame playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 33–58.
O'Laughlin, E., & Brubaker, B. (1998). Use of landmarks in cognitive mapping: Gender differences in self report versus performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 595–601.
Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotation Test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2001, February). The changing online population: It's more and more like the general population. Retrieved August, 2003, from http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/.
Provenzo, E. (1991). Video kids: Making sense of Nintendo. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rizzo, A., Buckwalter, J., Neumann, U., Chua, C., Van Rooyen, A., Larson, P., et al. (1999). Virtual environments for targeting cognitive processes: An overview of projects at the University of Southern California. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 2, 89–100.
Roberts, J., & Bell, M. (2000). Sex differences on a computerized mental rotation task disappear with computer familiarization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 1027–1034.
Rushbrook, S. (1986). The “message” of video games: Social implications. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47(6-B), 534B.
Saccuzzo, D., Craig, A., Johnson, N., & Larson, G. (1996). Gender differences in dynamic spatial abilities. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 599–607.
Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.
Sims, V., & Mayer, E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of the video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 97–115.
Stumpf, H., & Eliot, J. (1995). Gender-related differences in spatial ability and the k factor of general spatial ability in a population of academically talented students. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 33–45.
Subrahmanyam, K., & Greenfield, P. (1996). Effect of videogame practice on spatial skills in girls and boys. In P. Greenfield & R. Cocking (Eds.), Interacting with video (pp. 95–114). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Talbot, K. F., & Haude, R. H. (1993) The relation between sign language skill and spatial visualization ability: Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, 1387–1391.
Tracy, D. (1990). Toy-playing behavior, sex-role orientation, spatial ability, and science achievement. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27, 637–649.
Vandenberg, S., & Kuse, A. (1978). Mental rotations: A group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604.
Vandenberg, S., Kuse, A., & Vogler, G. (1985). Searching for correlates of spatial ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 343–350.
Voyer, D., Nolan, C., & Voyer, S. (2000). The relation between experience and spatial performance in men and women. Sex Roles, 43, 891–915.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Terlecki, M.S., Newcombe, N.S. How Important Is the Digital Divide? The Relation of Computer and Videogame Usage to Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability. Sex Roles 53, 433–441 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-6765-0
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-6765-0