Abstract
Concerns that the growing competition for funding and citations might distort science are frequently discussed, but have not been verified directly. Of the hypothesized problems, perhaps the most worrying is a worsening of positive-outcome bias. A system that disfavours negative results not only distorts the scientific literature directly, but might also discourage high-risk projects and pressure scientists to fabricate and falsify their data. This study analysed over 4,600 papers published in all disciplines between 1990 and 2007, measuring the frequency of papers that, having declared to have “tested” a hypothesis, reported a positive support for it. The overall frequency of positive supports has grown by over 22% between 1990 and 2007, with significant differences between disciplines and countries. The increase was stronger in the social and some biomedical disciplines. The United States had published, over the years, significantly fewer positive results than Asian countries (and particularly Japan) but more than European countries (and in particular the United Kingdom). Methodological artefacts cannot explain away these patterns, which support the hypotheses that research is becoming less pioneering and/or that the objectivity with which results are produced and published is decreasing.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.





References
Atkin, P. A. (2002). A paradigm shift in the medical literature. British Medical Journal, 325(7378), 1450–1451.
Bian, Z. X., & Wu, T. X. (2010). Legislation for trial registration and data transparency. Trials, 11, 64. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-11-64.
Bonitz, M., & Scharnhorst, A. (2001). Competition in science and the Matthew core journals. Scientometrics, 51(1), 37–54.
Browman, H. I. (1999). The uncertain position, status and impact of negative results in marine ecology: Philosphical and practical considerations. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 191, 301–309.
Csada, R. D., James, P. C., & Espie, R. H. M. (1996). The “file drawer problem” of non-significant results: Does it apply to biological research? Oikos, 76(3), 591–593.
de Meis, L., Velloso, A., Lannes, D., Carmo, M. S., & de Meis, C. (2003). The growing competition in Brazilian science: Rites of passage, stress and burnout. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 36(9), 1135–1141.
De Rond, M., & Miller, A. N. (2005). Publish or perish—Bane or boon of academic life? Journal of Management Inquiry, 14(4), 321–329. doi:10.1177/1056492605276850.
Delong, J. B., & Lang, K. (1992). Are all economic hypotheses false. Journal of Political Economy, 100(6), 1257–1272.
Doucouliagos, H., Laroche, P., & Stanley, T. D. (2005). Publication bias in union-productivity research? Relations Industrielles-Industrial Relations, 60(2), 320–347.
Dwan, K., Altman, D. G., Arnaiz, J. A., Bloom, J., Chan, A.-W., Cronin, E., et al. (2008). Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias. PLoS ONE, 3(8), e3081. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t; Review].
Evanschitzky, H., Baumgarth, C., Hubbard, R., & Armstrong, J. S. (2007). Replication research’s disturbing trend. Journal of Business Research, 60(4), 411–415. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.12.003.
Fanelli, D. (2010a). Do pressures to publish increase scientists’ bias? An empirical support from US States Data. Plos One, 5(4), e10271. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010271.
Fanelli, D. (2010b). “Positive” results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. Plos One, 5(3), e10068. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010068.
Feigenbaum, S., & Levy, D. M. (1996). Research bias: Some preliminary findings. Knowledge and Policy: The International Journal of Knowledge Transfer and Utilization, 9(2 & 3), 135–142.
Formann, A. K. (2008). Estimating the proportion of studies missing for meta-analysis due to publication bias. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 29(5), 732–739. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2008.05.004.
Fronczak, P., Fronczak, A., & Holyst, J. A. (2007). Analysis of scientific productivity using maximum entropy principle and fluctuation-dissipation theorem. Physical Review E, 75(2), 026103. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.75.026103.
Gad-el-Hak, M. (2004). Publish or perish—An ailing enterprise? Physics Today, 57(3), 61–62.
Gerber, A. S., & Malhotra, N. (2008). Publication bias in empirical sociological research—Do arbitrary significance levels distort published results? Sociological Methods & Research, 37(1), 3–30.
Howard, G. S., Hill, T. L., Maxwell, S. E., Baptista, T. M., Farias, M. H., Coelho, C., et al. (2009). What’s wrong with research literatures? And how to make them right. Review of General Psychology, 13(2), 146–166.
Hubbard, R., & Vetter, D. E. (1996). An empirical comparison of published replication research in accounting, economics, finance, management, and marketing. Journal of Business Research, 35(2), 153–164.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. Plos Medicine, 2(8), 696–701.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2006). Evolution and translation of research findings: From to where? Plos Clinical Trials, 1, e36. doi:10.1371/journal.pctr.0010036.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008a). Perfect study, poor evidence: Interpretation of biases preceding study design. Seminars in Hematology, 45(3), 160–166.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008b). Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology, 19(5), 640–648.
Ioannidis, J. P. A., Ntzani, E. E., Trikalinos, T. A., & Contopoulos-Ioannidis, D. G. (2001). Replication validity of genetic association studies. Nature Genetics, 29(3), 306–309.
Ioannidis, J. P. A., & Trikalinos, T. A. (2005). Early extreme contradictory estimates may appear in published research: The proteus phenomenon in molecular genetics research and randomized trials. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58(6), 543–549.
Jeng, M. (2006). A selected history of expectation bias in physics. American Journal of Physics, 74(7), 578–583.
Jennions, M. D., & Moller, A. P. (2002). Publication bias in ecology and evolution: An empirical assessment using the ‘trim and fill’ method. Biological Reviews, 77(2), 211–222.
Jennions, M. D., & Moller, A. P. (2003). A survey of the statistical power of research in behavioral ecology and animal behavior. Behavioral Ecology, 14(3), 438–445.
Jones, K. S., Derby, P. L., & Schmidlin, E. A. (2010). An investigation of the prevalence of replication research in human factors. Human Factors, 52(5), 586–595. doi:10.1177/0018720810384394.
Kelly, C. D. (2006). Replicating empirical research in behavioral ecology: How and why it should be done but rarely ever is. Quarterly Review of Biology, 81(3), 221–236.
King, D. A. (2004). The scientific impact of nations. Nature, 430(6997), 311–316. doi:10.1038/430311a.
Knight, J. (2003). Negative results: Null and void. Nature, 422(6932), 554–555.
Kundoor, V., & Ahmed, M. K. K. (2010). Uncovering negative results: Introducing an open access journal “Journal of Pharmaceutical Negative Results”. Pharmacognosy Magazine, 6(24), 345–347. doi:10.4103/0973-1296.71783.
Kyzas, P. A., Denaxa-Kyza, D., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2007). Almost all articles on cancer prognostic markers report statistically significant results. European Journal of Cancer, 43(17), 2559–2579.
Larsen, P. O., & von Ins, M. (2010). The rate of growth in scientific publication and the decline in coverage provided by Science Citation Index. Scientometrics, 84(3), 575–603. doi:10.1007/s11192-010-0202-z.
Lawrence, P. A. (2003). The politics of publication—Authors, reviewers and editors must act to protect the quality of research. Nature, 422(6929), 259–261. doi:10.1038/422259a.
Lortie, C. J. (1999). Over-interpretation: Avoiding the stigma of non-significant results. Oikos, 87(1), 183–184.
Maddock, J. E., & Rossi, J. S. (2001). Statistical power of articles published in three health psychology-related journals. Health Psychology, 20(1), 76–78.
Marsh, D. M., & Hanlon, T. J. (2007). Seeing what we want to see: Confirmation bias in animal behavior research. Ethology, 113(11), 1089–1098.
Meho, L. I. (2007). The rise and rise of citation analysis. Physics World, 20(1), 32–36.
Nicolini, C., & Nozza, F. (2008). Objective assessment of scientific performances world-wide. Scientometrics, 76(3), 527–541. doi:10.1007/s11192-007-1786-9.
Osuna, C., Crux-Castro, L., & Sanz-Menedez, L. (2011). Overturning some assumptions about the effects of evaluation systems on publication performance. Scientometrics, 86, 575–592.
Palmer, A. R. (2000). Quasireplication and the contract of error: Lessons from sex ratios, heritabilities and fluctuating asymmetry. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 31, 441–480.
Pautasso, M. (2010). Worsening file-drawer problem in the abstracts of natural, medical and social science databases. Scientometrics, 85(1), 193–202. doi:10.1007/s11192-010-0233-5.
Qiu, J. (2010). Publish or perish in China. Nature, 463(7278), 142–143. doi:10.1038/463142a.
Schmidt, S. (2009). Shall we really do it again? The powerful concept of replication is neglected in the social sciences. Review of General Psychology, 13(2), 90–100. doi:10.1037/a0015108.
Shelton, R. D., Foland, P., & Gorelskyy, R. (2007). Do new SCI journals have a different national bias? Proceedings of ISSI 2007: 11th international conference of the international society for scientometrics and informetrics, Vols I and II (pp. 708–717).
Shelton, R. D., Foland, P., & Gorelskyy, R. (2009). Do new SCI journals have a different national bias? Scientometrics, 79(2), 351–363. doi:10.1007/s11192-009-0423-1.
Silvertown, J., & McConway, K. J. (1997). Does “publication bias” lead to biased science? Oikos, 79(1), 167–168.
Simera, I., Moher, D., Hirst, A., Hoey, J., Schulz, K. F., & Altman, D. G. (2010). Transparent and accurate reporting increases reliability, utility, and impact of your research: Reporting guidelines and the EQUATOR Network. Bmc Medicine, 8, 24. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-8-24.
Song, F., Parekh, S., Hooper, L., Loke, Y. K., Ryder, J., Sutton, A. J., et al. (2010). Dissemination and publication of research findings: An updated review of related biases. Health Technology Assessment, 14(8), 1–193. doi:10.3310/hta14080.
Statzner, B., & Resh, V. H. (2010). Negative changes in the scientific publication process in ecology: Potential causes and consequences. Freshwater Biology, 55(12), 2639–2653. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2427.2010.02484.x.
Steen, R. G. (2011). Retractions in the scientific literature: Do authors deliberately commit research fraud? Journal of Medical Ethics, 37(2), 113–117.
Sterling, T. D., Rosenbaum, W. L., & Weinkam, J. J. (1995). Publication decisions revisited—The effect of the outcome of statistical tests on the decision to publish and vice versa. American Statistician, 49(1), 108–112.
Tsang, E. W. K., & Kwan, K. M. (1999). Replication and theory development in organizational science: A critical realist perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24(4), 759–780.
Warner, J. (2000). A critical review of the application of citation studies to the Research Assessment Exercises. Journal of Information Science, 26(6), 453–459.
Young, N. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., & Al-Ubaydi, O. (2008). Why current publication practices may distort science. Plos Medicine, 5(10), 1418–1422. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0050201.
Yousefi-Nooraie, R., Shakiba, B., & Mortaz-Hejri, S. (2006). Country development and manuscript selection bias: A review of published studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 6, 37.
Acknowledgments
Robin Williams gave helpful comments, and François Briatte crosschecked the coding protocol. This work was supported by a Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship (Grant Agreement Number PIEF-GA-2008-221441) and a Leverhulme Early-Career fellowship (ECF/2010/0131).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Fanelli, D. Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries. Scientometrics 90, 891–904 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-011-0494-7
Received:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-011-0494-7
Keywords
- Bias
- Misconduct
- Research evaluation
- Publication
- Publish or perish
- Competition