Abstract
We study the effects of three cognitive biases by the entrepreneur on obtaining funding. We find planning fallacy to increase funding amounts, whereas optimism and overconfidence by the entrepreneur have no effects on funding amounts from others. Further, planning fallacy positively impacts the probability of strong-tie (inside) investments but negatively impacts the probability of weak-tie (outside) investments. Mediation analyses further show that planning fallacy positively impacts venture performance through both self and other investor funding amounts. Our findings are not consistent with the pecking order theory of informal finance and suggest positive effects of at least one cognitive bias on entrepreneurial business success through increased funding.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
We cannot in this paper infer whether the entrepreneurs engage in self-deception or not. For an experimental paper on the role of overconfidence, self-deceptive beliefs, and persuading others, see Schwardmann and Van der Weele (2016).
An exception is a working paper by Dushnitsky (2009), which at a more aggregate level of analysis correlates country-level optimism with the country-level valuation of start-ups by venture capitalists. Also, Dushnitsky (2010) and Van den Steen (2004) have formally modeled the potential effects of entrepreneurial optimism on potential investors’ valuations of an entrepreneur’s venture ideas.
The CIC evaluates potential entrepreneurs and their venture ideas at an early stage and provides diagnostic feedback. For more information about the CIC and its evaluation process, see, for example, Åstebro and Koehler (2007).
All of the data except for the IAP evaluation were collected through a telephone survey. We developed a list of 6405 inventor-entrepreneurs who had submitted ideas for IAP review between 1994 and 2001. Of this number, we were able to trace 1352 current addresses. Of these, 1272 addresses led to actual contacts. The adjusted response rate was calculated by the Center as the contact rate (1272/1352) multiplied by the cooperation rate (830/1272). The Center follows the statistical methods and best practices of the American Association of Public Opinion Research, http://www.aapor.org. For further information about the survey procedure, please contact the authors.
To better understand the composition of the entrepreneur sample, we further draw a comparison sample from the general Canadian population. Using random digit dialing, we queried a sample of 300 Canadians from the general population based on sampling quotas for province, employment, and gender, to reflect the similarities in the aggregate with the entrepreneurs on these three variables. Comparisons are then made on background characteristics (results available upon request). The combined samples from the general population matched with the entrepreneurs contain unusually a high proportion reporting that they are self-employed (63 %) or that they have owned a business (60 %). However, the rate of entrepreneurship is much higher for the entrepreneur sample than it is for the general population sample.
Sales data are truncated by survey date for some observations. If data truncation is correlated with independent variables, coefficient estimates may be biased. In an alternate analysis, we therefore forecasted future sales that were conditional on truncation using exogenous parameters. This implies that the forecast was uncorrelated with covariates and should not produce biased regression parameters, but it may introduce more noise, thus increasing the standard errors. We used the Bass diffusion with exogenous parameters to forecast sales. Results of this analysis are consistent with those reported in text. Contact the authors for detailed methods and results.
Pretests results indicated respondents’ difficulty with the items “I feel blue’ and ‘I dislike myself.’ After careful deliberations on the possible effects of the two items on the questionnaire response and reliability, we decided to exclude these items and two other items that are matched with them—hence the 6 items out of the original 10 items.
While following the basic principles for establishing mediation as implemented for OLS (Baron and Kenny 1986), our tests are performed within a more general statistical estimation framework (see Clogg et al. 1995) as follows. The correlation between the focal independent variable and the dependent variable is estimated without the mediator, while including all covariates. The model is then re-estimated adding the mediator. This generates two sets of parameter vectors and variance–covariance matrices. A Chi-square test on the difference in the target coefficient between the two estimations, given the differences in the parameter vectors and variance–covariance matrix between the two estimations, is computed to test whether there is mediation. The suest routine in Stata is used for this purpose (Weesie 1999).
References
Arabsheibani, G. D., de Meza, D., Maloney, R. J., & Pearson, B. (2000). And a vision appeared unto them of a great profit: Evidence of self-deception among the self-employed. Economic Letters, 67, 35–41.
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 309–379.
Åstebro, T., Jeffrey, S. A., & Adomdza, G. (2007). Inventor perseverance after being told to quit: The role of cognitive biases. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(3), 253–272.
Åstebro, T., & Koehler, D. (2007). Calibration accuracy of a judgmental process that predicts the commercial success of new product ideas. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(4), 381–403.
Åstebro, T., & Serrano, C. (2015). Business partners: Complementary assets, financing and the commercialization of inventions. Journal of Economics and Management Strategy, 24(2), 228–252.
Baron, R. A. (1998). Cognitive mechanisms in entrepreneurship: Why and when entrepreneurs think differently than other people. Journal of Business Venturing, 13, 275–294.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Bohner, G., & Dickel, N. (2011). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 391–417.
Bohner, G., Dykema-Engblade, A., Tindale, R. S., & Meisenhelder, H. (2008a). Framing of majority and minority source information in persuasion. When and how “consensus implies correctness. Social Psychology, 39, 108–116.
Bohner, G., Erb, H. P., & Siebler, F. (2008b). Information processing approaches to persuasion integrating assumptions from the dual- and single-processing perspectives. In D. W. Crano & R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 161–188). New York: Psychology Press.
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Peetz, J. (2010). Chapter one—The planning fallacy: Cognitive, motivational, and social origins. In P. Z. Mark (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 43, pp. 1–62). Cambridge: Academic Press.
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the “planning fallacy”: Why people underestimate their completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 366–381.
Busenitz, L. W., & Barney, J. W. (1997). Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal of Business Venturing, 12(6), 9–30.
BusinessWeek. (2006). Think twice before borrowing from family, April 19, 2006.
Camerer, C. F., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental approach. American Economic Review, 89, 306–318.
Casamatta, C. (2003). Financing and advising: Optimal financial contracts with venture capitalists. Journal of Finance, 58, 2059–2086.
Cassar, G. (2010). Are individuals entering self-employment overly optimistic? An empirical test of plans and projections on nascent entrepreneur expectations. Strategic Management Journal, 31(8), 822–840.
Chen, X.-P., Xin, Y., & Kotha, S. (2009). Entrepreneur passion and preparedness in business plan presentation: A persuasion analysis of venture capitalists’ funding decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 52(1), 199–214.
Clogg, C. C., Petkova, E., & Haritou, A. (1995). Statistical methods for comparing regression coefficients between models. American Journal of Sociology, 100, 1269–1293.
Dew, N., Read, S., Sarasvathy, S., & Wiltbank, R. (2009). Effectual versus predictive logics in entrepreneurial decision-making: Differences between experts and novices. Journal of Business Venturing, 24(4), 287–309.
Dushnitsky, G. (2009). A cross-country study of entrepreneurial optimism and valuations. Working paper http://dushnitsky.com/research.html
Dushnitsky, G. (2010). Entrepreneurial optimism in the market for technology inventions. Organization Science, 21(1), 150–167.
Erb, H.-P., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Spiegel, S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2007). Biased processing of persuasive information: On the functional equivalence of cues and message arguments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1057–1075.
Evans, D. S., & Jovanovic, B. (1989). An estimated model of entrepreneurial choice under liquidity constraints. Journal of Political Economy, 97(4), 808–827.
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with Certainty: The Appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(4), 552–564.
Gibson, R., Tanner, C., & Wagner, A. F. (2014). Preferences for truthfulness: Heterogeneity among and within individuals. American Economic Review, 103, 532–548.
Graham, J. W., & Hofer, S. M. (2000). Multiple imputation in multivariate research. In T. D. Little, K. U. Schnabel, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multiple-group data: Practical issues, applied approaches, and specific examples (pp. 201–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Granovetter, M. (1974). Getting a job: A study of contacts and careers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Greene, W. H. (2003). Econometric analysis (5th ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Griffin, D., & Tversky, A. (1992). The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 411–435.
Hellmann, T., & Puri, M. (2002). Venture capital and the professionalization of start-up firms: Empirical evidence. Journal of Finance, 57(1), 169–197.
Hmieleski, K. M., & Baron, R. A. (2009). Entrepreneurs’ optimism and new venture performance: A social cognitive perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 52(3), 473–488.
Holt, D., Smith, M. F., & Winter, P. D. (1980). Regression analysis of data from complex surveys. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series A (General), 143(4), 474–487.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Penguin.
Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: A cognitive perspective on risk-taking. Management Science, 39, 17–31.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & P. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions: A reply to Gigerenzer’s critique. Psychological Review, 103, 582–591.
Keh, H. T., Foo, M. D., & Lim, B. C. (2002). Opportunity evaluation under risky conditions: The cognitive processes of entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship Theory & Practice, 27(2), 125–149.
Koellinger, P., Minniti, M., & Schade, C. (2007). I think I can, I think I can—Overconfidence and entrepreneurial behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(4), 502–507.
Lee, S., & Persson, P. (2015). Financing from family and friends. (September 10, 2015). IFN Working Paper No. 933; NYU Stern Working Paper FIN-12-007; ECGI—Finance Working Paper No. 358. http://ssrn.com/abstract=2086625.
Moore, D. A., & Healy, P. J. (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review, 115(2), 502–517.
Nagy, B. G., Pollack, J. M., Rutherford, M. W., & Lohrke, F. T. (2012). The influence of entrepreneurs’ credentials and impression management behaviors on perceptions of new venture legitimacy. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 36(5), 941–965.
Oregon Research Institute. (2001). A scientific collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of personality traits and other individual differences. http://ipip.ori.org/.
Oskamp, S. (1965). Overconfidence in case-study judgements. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 2, 261–265.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123–205.
Pollack, J. M., Rutherford, M. W., & Nagy, B. G. (2012). Preparedness and cognitive legitimacy as antecedents of new venture funding in televised business pitches. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 36(5), 915–939.
Puri, M., & Robinson, D. T. (2007). Optimism and economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, 86(1), 71–99.
Robinson, D., & Robb, A. (2014). The capital structure decisions of new firms. Review of Financial Studies, 27(1), 153–179.
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063.
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and psychological well-being. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 189–216). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Schwardmann, P., & Van der Weele, J. J. (2016). Deception and self-deception. Tinbergen Institute Discussion Paper, TI 2016-012/I. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2734736.
Shane, S., & Cable, D. (2002). Network ties, reputation, and the financing of new ventures. Management Science, 48(3), 364–381.
Shanteau, J. (1988). Psychological characteristics and strategies of expert decision makers. Acta Psychologica, 68(1–3), 203–215.
Shanteau, J. (1992). How much information does an expert use? Is it relevant? Acta Psychologica, 81(1), 75–86.
Simon, M., & Houghton, S. M. (2002). The relationship among biases, misperceptions and introducing pioneering products: Examining differences in venture decision contexts. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 27, 105–124.
Simon, M., & Shrader, R. C. (2012). Entrepreneurial actions and optimistic overconfidence: The role of motivated reasoning in new product introductions. Journal of Business Venturing, 27(3), 291–309.
Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53(4), 429–439.
Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, CA: The Benjamin-Cummings Publishing Company.
Van Buuren, S., Boshuizen, H. C., & Knook, D. L. (1999). Multiple imputation of missing blood pressure covariates in survival analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 18, 681–694.
Van den Steen, E. (2004). Rational overoptimism (and other biases). The American Economic Review, 94(4), 1141–1151.
Von Hippel, W., & Trivers, R. (2011). The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(1), 1–16.
Wanous, J. P., & Hudy, M. J. (2001). Single-item reliability: A replication and extension. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 361–375.
Weesie, J. (1999). Seemingly unrelated estimation and the cluster-adjusted sandwich estimator. STATA Technical Bulletin, STB, 52, 34–47.
Wong, A., Bhatia, M., & Freeman, Z. Freeman. (2009). Angel finance: The other venture capital. Strategic Change, 18, 221–230.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Adomdza, G.K., Åstebro, T. & Yong, K. Decision biases and entrepreneurial finance. Small Bus Econ 47, 819–834 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11187-016-9739-4
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11187-016-9739-4
Keywords
- Entrepreneurship
- Decision biases
- Cognitive biases
- Entrepreneurial finance
- Informal finance
- Fundraising
- Social ties
- Venture performance