Skip to main content
Log in

Data Generation in the Discovery Sciences—Learning from the Practices in an Advanced Research Laboratory

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

General scientific literacy includes understanding the grounds on which scientific claims are based. The measurements scientists make and the data that they produce from them generally constitute these grounds. However, the nature of data generation has received relatively little attention from those interested in teaching science through inquiry. To inform curriculum designers about the process of data generation and its relation to the understanding of patterns as these may arise from graphs, this 5-year ethnographic study in one advanced research laboratory was designed to investigate how natural scientists make decisions about the inclusion/exclusion of certain measurements in/from their data sources. The study shows that scientists exclude measurements from their data sources even before attempting to mathematize and interpret the data. The excluded measurements therefore never even enter the ground from and against which the scientific phenomenon emerges and therefore remain invisible to it. I conclude by encouraging science educators to squarely address this aspect of the discovery sciences in their teaching, which has both methodological and ethical implications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aberg-Bengtsson, L., & Ottosson, T. (2006). What lies behind graphicacy? Relating students’ results on a test of graphically represented graphical information to formal academic achievement. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43, 43–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akerson, V., & Donnelly, L. A. (2010). Teaching nature of science to K–2 students: what understandings can they attain? International Journal of Science Education, 32, 97–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G., Sweeting, R., & McKeown, B. (1994). The shift in visual pigment dominance in the retinae of juvenile coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch): an indicator of smolt status. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 195, 185–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apedoe, X., & Ford, M. (2010). The empirical attitude, material practice and design activities. Science Education, 19, 165–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlone, H. B., Haun-Frank, J., & Webb, A. (2011). Assessing equity beyond knowledge- and skills-based outcomes: a comparative ethnography of two fourth-grade reform-based science classrooms. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 459–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chin, C., & Chia, L.-G. (2004). Problem-based learning: using students’ questions to drive knowledge construction. Science Education, 88, 707–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (2001). Models of data: a theory of how people evaluate data. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 323--393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Tzou, C. (2009). Supporting students’ learning about data generation. In W.-M. Roth (Ed.), Mathematical representation at the interface of body and culture (pp. 135–170). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coy, M. (1989). Being what we pretend to be: The usefulness of apprenticeship as a field method. In M. W. Coy (Ed.), Apprenticeship: from theory to method and back again (pp. 115–135). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1995). The construction of lay expertise: AIDS activism and the forging of credibility in the reform of clinical trials. Science, Technology, & Human Values, 20, 408–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1997). Activism, drug regulation, and the politics of therapeutic evaluation in the AIDS era: a case study of ddC and the ‘Surrogate Markers’ debate. Social Studies of Science, 27, 691–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, H., Brill, G., & Yarden, A. (2008). Teaching a biotechnology curriculum based on adapted primary literature. International Journal of Science Education, 30, 1841–1866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, A., & Pirog, K. (2011). Authentic science research in elementary school after-school science clubs. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20, 494–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogleman, J., McNeill, K. L., & Krajcik, J. (2011). Examining the effect of teachers’ adaptations of a middle school science inquiry-oriented curriculum unit on student learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 149–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1996). Ethnomethodology’s program. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 5–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (2002). Ethnomethodology’s program: working out Durkheim’s aphorism. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H., Lynch, M., & Livingston, E. (1981). The work of a discovering science construed with materials from the optically discovered pulsar. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 11, 131–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H., & Sacks, H. (1986). On formal structures of practical action. In H. Garfinkel (Ed.), Ethnomethodological studies of work (pp. 160–193). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garli, B., & Rule, A. C. (2009). Integrating social justice with mathematics and science: an analysis of student teacher lessons. Teaching and Teacher Education, 25, 490–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Govardovskii, V. I., Fyhrquist, N., Reuter, T., Kuzmin, D. G., & Donner, K. (2000). In search of the visual pigment template. Visual Neuroscience, 17, 509–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hárosi, F. I. (1987). Cynomolgus and Rhesus monkey visual pigment: application of fourier transform smoothiung and statistical techniques to the determination of spectral parameters. Journal of General Physiology, 89, 717–743.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, R. C., Ruibai-Villasenor, M., Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Etkina, E. (2011). Laboratory materials: affordances or constraints? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 1010–1025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: how to follow scientists and engineers through society. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993). La clef de Berlin et autres leçons d’un amateur de sciences [The key to Berlin and other lessons of a science lover]. Paris: Éditions la Découverte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, R. (2011). Science education from people for people: taking a standpoint. Studies in Science Education, 47, 109–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1990). The externalized retina: Selection and mathematization in the visual documentation of objects in the life sciences. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 153–186). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElhaney, K. W., & Linn, M. C. (2011). Investigations of a complex, realistic task: intentional, unsystematic, and exhaustive experiments. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 745–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medical Billing and Coding. (2010). Sitting is killing you. Accessed February 1, 2012 at http://www.medicalbillingandcoding.org/sitting-kills/

  • Metz, K. E. (2004). Children’s understanding of scientific inquiry: their conceptualization of uncertainty in investigations of their own design. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 219–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munz, F. W., & Beatty, D. D. (1965). A critical analysis of the visual pigments of salmon and trout. Vision Research, 5, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, G. (1990). Every picture tells a story: illustrations in E. O. Wilson’s Sociobiology. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 231–265). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nargund-Joshi, V., Park Rogers, M. A., & Akerson, V. L. (2011). Exploring Indian secondary teachers’ orientations and practice for teaching science in an era of reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 624–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, S. M., Tobin, K., Sandhu, M., Sandhu, S., Henderson, S., & Roth, W.-M. (2012). Emotional arousal of beginning physics teachers during extended experimental investigations. Journal of Research in Science Teaching.

  • Roth, W.-M. (1996). Where is the context in contextual word problems?: mathematical practices and products in Grade 8 students’ answers to story problems. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 487–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2001). “Authentic science”: enculturation into the conceptual blind spots of a discipline. British Educational Research Journal, 27, 5–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2003). Towards an anthropology of graphing: activity theoretic and semiotic perspectives. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2004). “Tappen Im Dunkeln”. Der Umgang mit Unsicherheiten und Unwägbarkeiten während des Forschungsprozesses [Groping in the dark. Dealing with uncertainties during scientific research]. Zeitschrift für Qualitative Bildungs-, Beratungs-, und Sozialforschung, 5(2), 155–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2005a). Doing qualitative research: praxis of methods. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2005b). Making classifications (at) work: ordering practices in science. Social Studies of Science, 35, 581–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2008). Constructing community health and safety. Municipal Engineer, 161, 83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2009). Radical uncertainty in scientific discovery work. Science, Technology & Human Values, 34, 313–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2012). Undoing decontextualization or how scientists come to understand their own data/graphs. Science Education.

  • Roth, W.-M., & Barton, A. C. (2004). Rethinking scientific literacy. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (1999a). Complexities of graphical representations during ecology lectures: an analysis rooted in semiotics and hermeneutic phenomenology. Learning and Instruction, 9, 235–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (1999b). Digitizing lizards or the topology of vision in ecological fieldwork. Social Studies of Science, 29, 719–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (2001). “Creative solutions” and “fibbing results”: enculturation in field ecology. Social Studies of Science, 31, 533–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (2003). When are graphs ten thousand words worth? An expert/expert study. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 429–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., Hawryshyn, C., Haimberger, T., & Welzel, M. (2001). Visual perception: more than meets the eye. Fribourg: Paper presented at the bi-annual meeting of the European Association for Research on Learning and instruction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., Lee, Y. J., & Boyer, L. (2008). The eternal return: reproduction and change in complex activity systems. The case of salmon enhancement. Berlin: Lehmanns Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & McGinn, M. K. (1997). Graphing: a cognitive ability or cultural practice? Science Education, 81, 91–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., McGinn, M. K., & Bowen, G. M. (1998). How prepared are preservice teachers to teach scientific inquiry? Levels of performance in scientific representation practices. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 9, 25–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, C. B., & Weaver, G. C. (2011). A comparative study of traditional, inquiry-based, and research-based laboratory curricula: impacts on understanding of the nature of science. Chemistry Education Research and Practice, 12, 57–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadeh, I., & Zion, M. (2009). The development of dynamic inquiry performances within an open inquiry setting: a comparison to guided inquiry setting. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46, 1137–1160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, K., Timms, M., Moorjani, A., Clark, L., Holtermann, K., & Irvin, P. S. (2011). Student learning in science simulations: design features that promote learning gains. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 1050–1078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, P., & Freedman, E. G. (2011). Bar and line graph comprehension: an interaction of top-down and bottom-up processes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 560–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, S. E., Ramsden, S. D., Haimberger, T. J., Veldhoen, K. M., Veldhoen, N. J., Carter, N. L., et al. (2008). Effects of exogenous thyroid hormones on visual pigment composition in coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 2134–2143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, S. E., Plate, E. M., Ramsden, S., Haimberger, T. J., Roth, W.-M., & Hawryshyn, C. W. (2006). Seasonal cycle in vitamin A1/A2-based visual pigment composition during the life history of coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 192, 301–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, S. E., Veldhoen, K. M., Phelan, J. T., Veldhoen, N. J., & Hawryshyn, C. W. (2008). Ontogenetic changes in photoreceptor opsin gene expression in coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch, Walbaum). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 3879–3888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ten Have, P. (1999). Doing conversation analysis: a practical guide. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Valk, T., & de Jong, O. (2009). Scaffolding science teachers in open-inquiry teaching. International Journal of Science Education, 31, 829–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1989). Concrete human psychology. Soviet Psychology, 27(2), 53–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waight, N., & Abd-El-Khalick, F. (2011). From scientific practice to high school science classrooms: transfer of scientific technologies and realizations of authentic inquiry. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 37–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

A joint grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities (SSHRC) and Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Councils of Canada (NSERC) funded both the natural science and social science parts on which this research is based. I am grateful to my team members Craig Hawryshyn, Theodore (von) Haimberger, Elmar Plate, and Shelby Temple for their contributions to our collaborative project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wolff-Michael Roth.

Appendix

Appendix

For the transcriptions, I follow a commonly used system based on conversation analysis adapted for the inclusion of prosodic features (Selting et al., 1998). In the rules implemented here, everything is written in small letters and sound words that run into each other are transcribed that way unless the run-in sign “=” is used when it would be difficult to distinguish pronunciation (e.g., “a=one”). The transcription is phonetic such that if a participant pronounces the words “this” or “that” in the way a French or German speaker often does, that is, with a soft “d” or “s,” the transcription will read something like “ze other one is to read dis ze whole branch.”

figure g

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Roth, WM. Data Generation in the Discovery Sciences—Learning from the Practices in an Advanced Research Laboratory. Res Sci Educ 43, 1617–1644 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-012-9324-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-012-9324-z

Keywords

Navigation