Contributions of morphological awareness and rapid automatized naming (RAN) to Chinese children’s reading comprehension versus reading fluency: evidence from a longitudinal mediation model

Abstract

Some cognitive processes may be shared between reading comprehension and reading fluency, while others may be independent. In this longitudinal study, 127 Chinese children in grades 1–2 were tested three times (T1–T3) to explore the contributions of early morphological awareness and rapid automatized naming (RAN) to subsequent reading comprehension and reading fluency. The results demonstrated that both T1 morphological awareness and RAN directly and significantly explained a similar amount of variance in T3 reading comprehension and reading fluency. T1 Morphological awareness predicted T3 reading comprehension via T2 word-reading accuracy but not word-reading fluency, whereas the indirect influence of T1 RAN on T3 reading comprehension was not significant. In contrast, morphological awareness predicted reading fluency via word-reading accuracy and fluency, while RAN predicted it only via word-reading fluency not accuracy. The findings add to the growing literature by providing evidence of the similarities and differences in the cognitive underpinnings of reading comprehension versus reading fluency in Chinese children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Fig. 1

References

  1. Araújo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 868–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Berends, I. E., & Reitsma, P. (2006). Addressing semantics promotes the development of reading fluency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(2), 247–265. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716406060279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Caramazza, A., Laudanna, A., & Romani, C. (1988). Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 28(3), 297–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(88)90017-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12(3), 169–190. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008131926604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 464–487. https://doi.org/10.2307/20779541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cheng, Y., Li, L., & Wu, X. (2015). The reciprocal relationship between compounding awareness and vocabulary knowledge in Chinese: A latent growth model study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 440. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00440.

  7. Cheng, Y., Zhang, J., Li, H., Wu, X., Liu, H., Dong, Q., et al. (2017). Growth of compounding awareness predicts reading comprehension in young Chinese students: A longitudinal study from grade 1 to grade 2. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1), 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Cheng, Y., Zhang, J., Wu, X., Liu, H., & Li, H. (2016). Cross-lagged relationships between morphological awareness and reading comprehension among Chinese children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, article 1379. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01379.

  9. Cohen-Mimran, R. (2009). The contribution of language skills to reading fluency: A comparison of two orthographies for Hebrew. Journal of Child Language, 36(3), 657–672. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908009148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 558–577. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Collins, W. M., & Levy, B. A. (2008). Developing fluent text processing with practice: Memorial influences on fluency and comprehension. Canadian Psychology, 49(2), 133–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Deacon, S. H., Kieffer, M. J., & Laroche, A. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(6), 432–451. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.926907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Deacon, S. H., Tong, X., & Francis, K. (2017). The relationship of morphological analysis and morphological decoding to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18(2), 116–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00077.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2004). Monitoring early reading development in first grade: Word identification fluency versus nonsense word fluency. Exceptional Children, 71(1), 7–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290407100101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239–256. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Garcia, J. R., & Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Review of Educational Research, 84(1), 74–111. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654313499616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Georgiou, G. K., Aro, M., Liao, C., & Parrila, R. (2016). Modeling the relationship between rapid automatized naming and literacy skills across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 48–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(2), 199–220. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1002_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Geva, E., & Farnia, F. (2012). Developmental changes in the nature of language proficiency and reading fluency paint a more complex view of reading comprehension in ELL and EL1. Reading and Writing, 25(8), 1819–1845. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9333-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Goodwin, A. P., Huggins, A. C., Carlo, M., August, D., & Calderon, M. (2013). Minding morphology: How morphological awareness relates to reading for English language learners. Reading and Writing, 26(9), 1387–1415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9412-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258600700104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ho, C. S., Chan, D. W., Lee, S., Tsang, S., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91(1), 43–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00163-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00401799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hudson, R. F., Lane, H. B., & Pullen, P. C. (2005). Reading fluency assessment and instruction: What, why, and how? The Reading Teacher, 58(8), 702–714. https://doi.org/10.1598/RT.58.8.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hudson, R. F., Pullen, P. C., Lane, H. B., & Torgesen, J. K. (2009). The complex nature of reading fluency: A multidimensional view. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(1), 4–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573560802491208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Institute of Language Teaching and Research [of China]. (1986). A frequency dictionary of modern Chinese. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., Den Broek, P. V., Espin, C. A., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 719–729. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Johnston, T. C., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). The contribution of naming speed to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 19(4), 339–361. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-4644-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21(2), 85–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710050084428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Kieffer, M. J., Biancarosa, G., & Mancillamartinez, J. (2013). Roles of morphological awareness in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking language minority learners: Exploring partial mediation by vocabulary and reading fluency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(4), 697–725. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2012). Direct and indirect roles of morphological awareness in the English reading comprehension of native English, Spanish, Filipino, and Vietnamese speakers. Language Learning, 62(4), 1170–1204. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00722.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kim, Y. G. (2015). Developmental, component-based model of reading fluency: An investigation of predictors of word-reading fluency, text-reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(4), 459–481. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wadewoolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 389–410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 230–251. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.2.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41(3), 161–180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2017). Study on morphological awareness and rapid automatized naming through word reading and comprehension in normal and disabled reading Arabic-speaking children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 33(2), 123–140. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2015.1105763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Lei, L., Pan, J., Liu, H., Mcbridechang, C., Li, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2011). Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 212–220. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02311.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). Morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Examining mediating factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Li, H., Shu, H., Mcbridechang, C., Liu, H., & Peng, H. (2012). Chinese children’s character recognition: Visuo-orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 287–307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Li, L., & Wu, X. (2015). Effects of metalinguistic awareness on reading comprehension and the mediator role of reading fluency from grades 2 to 4. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e114417. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Liao, C., Deng, C., Hamilton, J., Lee, C. S., Wei, W., & Georgiou, G. K. (2015). The role of rapid naming in reading development and dyslexia in Chinese. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 106–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Liao, C., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing, 21(3), 231–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9071-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Liu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 62–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Liu, P. D., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, T. T., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M. (2013). Morphological awareness in Chinese: Unique associations of homophone awareness and lexical compounding to word reading and vocabulary knowledge in Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(4), 755–775. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641200001X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Liu, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Zhang, Y., Hong, L., Liu, H., Song, S., et al. (2017). Contribution of cognitive and linguistic skills to word-reading accuracy and fluency in Chinese. International Journal of Educational Research, 82, 75–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2016.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Luo, Y. C., Chen, X., Deacon, S. H., Zhang, J., & Yin, L. (2012). The role of visual processing in learning to read Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 22–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.689790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. McBride, C. A. (2016). Is Chinese special? Four aspects of Chinese literacy acquisition that might distinguish learning Chinese from learning alphabetic orthographies. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 523–549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9318-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. McBride-Chang, C., Lam, F., Lam, C., Chan, B., Fong, C. Y. C., Wong, T. T. Y., et al. (2011). Early predictors of dyslexia in Chinese children: Familial history of dyslexia, language delay, and cognitive profiles. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 204–211. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02299.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 743–751. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J., Shu, H., Fletcher, P. C., Stokes, S. F., et al. (2008). What’s in a word? Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(3), 437–462. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640808020X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 427–452. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Barros, R. (2012). The development of word recognition and its significance for comprehension and fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 959–973. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Pan, J., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, H. (2011). What is in the naming? A 5-year longitudinal study of early rapid naming and phonological sensitivity in relation to subsequent reading skills in both native Chinese and English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 897–908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Pan, J., Song, S., Su, M., McBride, C., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2016). On the relationship between phonological awareness, morphological awareness and Chinese literacy skills: evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 19(6), 982–991. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Gottardo, A., & Geva, E. (2015). Cross-language transfer of word reading accuracy and word reading fluency in Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals: Script-universal and script-specific processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 96–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430701530730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(4), 461–469. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Perfetti, C. A., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227–247). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Perfetti, C. A., & Stafura, J. Z. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.827687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(2), 180–202. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Shu, H. (2003). Chinese writing system and learning to read. International Journal of Psychology, 38(5), 274–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. C. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 886–899. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.37.6.886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2013). Children’s reading impairments: From theory to practice. Japanese Psychological Research, 55(2), 186–202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00541.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Nation, K. (1997). A connectionist perspective on the development of reading skills in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 88–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(97)89053-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Song, S., Georgiou, G. K., Su, M., & Shu, H. (2016). How well do phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming correlate with Chinese reading accuracy and fluency? A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(2), 99–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1088543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Song, S., Su, M., Kang, C., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., Mcbride-Chang, C., et al. (2015). Tracing children’s vocabulary development from preschool through the school-age years: An 8-year longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 18(1), 119–131. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Powell, D., Quinlan, P. T., & Garwood, H. (2010). Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performance. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(3), 266–292. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888431003724070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16(1), 32–71. https://doi.org/10.2307/747348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2018). Investigating phonological awareness and naming speed as predictors of reading in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 70–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1340948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and spelling errors: Keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(5), 426–452. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430903162910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2017). Unpacking the relation between morphological awareness and Chinese word reading: Levels of morphological awareness and vocabulary. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011a). Introduction to this special issue: Vocabulary growth and reading skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.536124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011b). Morphological processing in reading acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(3), 457–466. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Wang, M., Cheng, C., & Chen, S. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to chinese-english biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 542–553. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Wei, W., Georgiou, G. K., & Deng, C. (2015). Examining the cross-lagged relationships between RAN and word reading in Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(6), 446–455. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1077447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Weng, X., Li, G., & Li, R. (2015). Mediating effects of working Memory in the relation between Rapid Automatized Naming and Chinese reading comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45(4), 945–959. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-015-9385-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Wolf, M. (1984). Naming, reading, and the dyslexias: A longitudinal overview. Annals of Dyslexia, 34(1), 87–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02663615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Wu, X., Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Wu, X., Li, H., Zhang, J., et al. (2009). Morphological awareness and Chinese children’s literacy development: An intervention study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(1), 26–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Xue, J., Shu, H., Li, H., Li, W., & Tian, X. (2013). The stability of literacy-related cognitive contributions to Chinese character naming and reading fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42(5), 433–450. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9228-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Yeung, P., Ho, C. S., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K., & Wong, Y. (2013). A model of reading comprehension in Chinese elementary school children. Learning and Individual Differences, 25(3), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Zhang, H. C., & Wang, X. P. (1985). Chinese version of Raven’s IQ reasoning standardized test. Beijing: Beijing Normal University Press. (in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  89. Zhang, J., Mcbride-Chang, C., Tong, X., Wong, A. M. Y., Shu, H., & Fong, C. Y. C. (2012). Reading with meaning: the contributions of meaning-related variables at the word and subword levels to early Chinese reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 25(9), 2183–2203. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9353-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading a cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all the principals, teachers, children and their parents for their participation. This research was supported by Major Project of the National Social Science Fund of China (13&ZD188) and by Major Project of Philosophy and Social Science Foundation by Ministry of Education (11JZD041).

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xinchun Wu.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhao, Y., Cheng, Y. & Wu, X. Contributions of morphological awareness and rapid automatized naming (RAN) to Chinese children’s reading comprehension versus reading fluency: evidence from a longitudinal mediation model. Read Writ 32, 2013–2036 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09935-w

Download citation

Keywords

  • Reading comprehension
  • Reading fluency
  • Morphological awareness
  • Rapid automatized naming (RAN)
  • Word-reading skills