Skip to main content
Log in

Unpacking pathways using the direct and indirect effects model of writing (DIEW) and the contributions of higher order cognitive skills to writing

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined the Direct and Indirect Effects model of Writing (DIEW), using longitudinal data from Korean-speaking beginning writers. DIEW posits hierarchical structural relations among component skills (e.g., transcription skills, higher order cognitive skills, oral language, motivation/affect, background knowledge) where lower level skills are needed for higher order skills and where component skills make direct and indirect contributions to writing (see Fig. 1). A total of 201 Korean-speaking children were assessed on component skills in Grade 1, including transcription (spelling and handwriting fluency), higher order cognitive skills (inference, perspective taking, and monitoring), oral language (vocabulary and grammatical knowledge), and executive function (working memory and attention). Their writing skills were assessed in Grades 1 and 3. DIEW fit the data well. In Grade 1, transcription skills were directly related to writing, whereas vocabulary, grammatical knowledge, working memory, and attention were indirectly related to writing. For Grade 3 writing, inference and spelling were directly related while working memory made both direct and indirect contributions. Attention, vocabulary, and grammatical knowledge made indirect contributions via spelling and inference. These results support DIEW and its associated hypotheses such as the hierarchical nature of structural relations, the roles of higher order cognitive skills, and the changing relations of component skills to writing as a function of development (a developmental hypothesis).

Direct and indirect effects model of writing (DIEW)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary- and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alonzo, C. N., Yeomans-Maldonado, C., Murphy, K. A., Bevens, B., & LARRC. (2016). Predicting second grade listening comprehension using prekindergarten measures. Topics in Language Disorders, 36, 3123–3333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Applebee, A. N. (1984). Writing and reasoning. Review of Educational Research, 54, 577–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arfe, B., Dockrell, J. E., & De Bernardi, B. (2016). The effect of language-specific factors on early written composition: The role of spelling, oral language and text generation skills in a shallow orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 501–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621–635. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J., & Jenkins, J. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S. (2014). Contributions of word-level and verbal skills to written expression: Comparison of learners who speak English as a first (L1) and second language (L2). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1207–1229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2010). Component processes of early reading, spelling, and narrative writing skills in Turkish: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 539–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1984). Children’s effective use of multiple standards for evaluating their comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 588–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaufort, A. (2004). Developmental gains of a history major: A case for building a theory of disciplinary writing expertise. Research in the Teaching of English, 39, 136–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2010). Discourse-level oral language, oral expression, reading comprehension, and written expression: Related yet unique language systems in grades 1, 3, 5, and 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 635–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Vaughn, K. B., Graham, S., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Rogan, L. W., et al. (1997). Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 652–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Winn, W. D. (2006). Implications of advancements in brain research and technology for writing development, writing instruction, and educational evolution. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 96–114). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Yates, C. W., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 257–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bianco, M., Bressoux, P., Doyen, A. L., Lambert, E., Lima, L., Pellenq, C., et al. (2010). Early training in oral comprehension and phonological skills: Results of a three-year longitudinal Study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 211–246. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430903117518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review, 89, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdin, B., & Fayol, M. (1994). Is written language production more difficult than oral language production? A working memory approach. International Journal of Psychology, 29, 591–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caillies, S., & Sourn-Bissaoui, S. (2008). Children’s understanding of idioms and theory of mind development. Developmental Science, 11, 703–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, J. B. (2002). Developing audience awareness in writing. Journal of Research in Reading, 25, 271–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, J.-R. (2009). Syllable and letter knowledge in earlier Korean Hangul reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 938–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, J.-R., McBride-Chang, C., & Park, S.-G. (2008). Phonological awareness and morphological awareness: Differential associations to regular and irregular word recognition in early Korean Hangul readers. Reading and Writing, 21, 255–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coker, D. L. (2006). Impact of first-grade factors on the growth and outcomes of urban schoolchildren’s primary-grade writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 471–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2012). Whichever way you choose to categorize, working memory helps you learn. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 439–464. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.608854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Temple, J. M., Wu, S.-F., & Snow, C. E. (1991). Papa pig just left for pigtown: Children’s oral and written picture descriptions under varying instructions. Discourse Processes, 14, 469–495. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539109544797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dromi, E., & Berman, R. A. (1986). Language-specific and language-general in developing syntax. Journal of Child Language, 13, 371–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105–121). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbow, P. (1981). Writing with power. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoè, G., & Levorato, M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 935–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C. (2014). Listening text comprehension in preschoolers: A longitudinal study on the role of semantic components. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 793–817. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9464-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Berninger, V., & Fan, W. (2007). The structural relationship between writing attitude and writing achievement in first and third grade students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 516–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2017). Reading and writing connections: How writing can build better readers (and vice versa). In Improving reading and reading engagement in the 21st century (pp. 333–350). Singapore: Springer.

  • Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Chorzempa, B. F. (2002). Contribution of spelling instruction to the spelling, writing, and reading of poor spellers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 669–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & McKeown, D. (2013). The writing of students with LD and a meta-analysis of SRSD writing intervention studies: Redux. In L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (2nd ed.). MY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gwon, E.-Y., & Lee, H.-J. (2012). Children’s development of lying, false belief and executive function. Korean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 165–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R. (2006). New directions in writing theory. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 28–40). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R., & Chenoweth, N. A. (2007). Working memory in an editing task. Written Communication, 24, 283–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hooper, S. R., Costa, L.-J., McBee, M., Anderson, K. L., Yerby, D. C., Knuth, S. B., et al. (2011). Concurrent and longitudinal neuropsychological contributors to written language expression in first and second grade students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 221–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children with autism to mind read. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, S., Wanzek, J., Petscher, Y., Al Otaiba, S., & Kim, Y.-S. (2014). Writing fluency and quality in kindergarten and first grade: The role of attention, reading, transcription, and oral language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1163–1188. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9480-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2011). Considering linguistic and orthographic features in early literacy acquisition: Evidence from Korean. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 177–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.06.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2015a). Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development, 86, 128–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, L., & Puranik, C. (2014a). Evaluating the dimensionality of first grade written composition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0152).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Puranik, C., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, L., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Componential skills of beginning writing: An exploratory study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 517–525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Wanzek, J., & Gatlin, B. (2015a). Towards an understanding of dimension, predictors, and gender gaps in written composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 79–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Apel, K., & Al Otaiba, S. (2013a). The relation of linguistic awareness and vocabulary to word reading and spelling for first-grade students participating in response to instruction. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2013/12-0013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Park, C., & Park, Y. (2013b). Is academic language use a separate dimension in begining writing? Evidence from Korean children. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.06.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Park, C., & Wagner, R. K. (2014b). Is oral/text reading fluency a “bridge” to reading comprehension? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 79–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9434-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., & Petscher, Y. (2013). Considering word characteristics for spelling accuracy: Evidence from Korean-speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 80–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., & Phillips, B. (2014). Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 269–281. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G. (2015b). Developmental, component-based model of reading fluency: An investigation of word-reading fluency, text-reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 459–481. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension or oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G. (2017a). Why the simple view of reading is not simplistic: Unpacking the simple view of reading using a direct and indirect effect model of reading (DIER). Scientific Studies of Reading, 21, 310–333. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1291643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G. (2017b). Multicomponent view of vocabulary acquisition: An investigation with primary grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 120–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G. (in press). Interactive dynamic literacy model: An integrative theoretical framework for reading and writing relations. In R. Alves, T. Limpo, & M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading-writing connections: Towards integrative literacy science. Netherlands: Springer.

  • Kim, Y.–S. G., Cho, J.-R., & Park, S.-G. (in press). The Korean Test of Language and Literacy Diagnosis (K-TOLLD). Korea Guidance. [in Korean]

  • Kim, Y.-S. G., Park, C., & Park, Y. (2015b). Dimensions of discourse-level oral language skills and their relations to reading comprehension and written composition: An exploratory study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28, 633–654. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9542-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. G., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 35–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klassen, R. (2002). Writing in early adolescence: A review of the role of self-efficacy beliefs. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 173–203. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014626805572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Laakkonen, E., Silvén, M., & Niemi, P. (2012). The role of inference making and other language skills in the development of narrative discourse-level oral language in 4- to 6-year old children. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 259–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limpo, T., & Alves, R. A. (2013). Modeling writing development: Contribution of transcription and self-regulation to Portuguese students’ text generation quality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 401–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2010). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1986). Domain knowledge and linguistic knowledge in the development of writing ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 431–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596x(86)90036-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge, processing, and working memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational Psychologist, 35, 13–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2013). Mplus 7.1. Los Angeles: Muthén and Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naigles, L. R., & Leher, N. (2002). Language-general and language-specific influences on children’s acquisition of argument structure: A comparison of French and English. Journal of Child Language, 29, 545–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Center for Education Statistics. (2012). The nation’s report card: Writing 2011(NCES 2012–470). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

  • Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 383–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olinghouse, N. G. (2008). Student- and instruction-level predictors of narrative writing in third-grade students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olinghouse, N. G., & Graham, S. (2009). The relationship between discourse knowledge and the writing performance of elementary-grade students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 37–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olinghouse, N. G., Graham, S., & Gillespie, A. (2015). The relationship of discourse and topic knowledge to fifth graders’ writing performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 391–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, T. (2004). Working memory in writing: Empirical evidence from the dual-task technique. European Psychologist, 9, 32–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pajares, F. (2003). Self-efficacy beliefs, motivation, and achievement in writing: A review of the literature. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 139–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573560308222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perin, D., Keselman, A., & Monopoli, M. (2003). The academic writing of community college remedial students: Text and learner variables. Higher Education, 45, 19–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radach, R., Kennedy, A., & Rayner, K. (2004). Eye movements and information processing during reading. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, P. M., Vander Lei, E. U., & Roen, D. H. (1999). Audience considerations for evaluating writing. In C. R. Cooper & L. Odell (Eds.), Evaluating writing (pp. 93–113). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sáez, L., Folsom, J. S., Al Otaiba, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). Relations among student attention behaviors, teacher practices, and beginning word reading skill. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 418–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411431243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scadamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1985). Development of dialectical processes in composition. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, H., & Ruddell, R. (Eds.). (1985). Theoretical models and processes of reading (3rd ed.). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, J. M., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, K., Sergeant, J. A., et al. (2006). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: The SNAP and SWAN rating scales. Retrieved from http://www.adhd.net/SNAP_SWAN.pdf.

  • Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language in the preschool years. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 403–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valle, A., Massaro, D., Castelli, I., & Marchetti, A. (2015). Theory of mind development in adolescence and early adulthood: The growing complexity of recursive thinking ability. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11, 112–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 65–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Puranik, C. S., Foorman, B., Foster, E., Tschinkel, E., & Kantor, P. T. (2011). Modeling the development of written language. Reading and Writing, 24, 203–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wollman-Bonilla, J. E. (2001). Can first-grade writers demonstrate audience awareness? Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 184–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Funding was provided by Institute of Education Sciences (Grant No. R305A130131) and National Research Foundation of Korea (Grant No. NRF-2013S1A3A2054928).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Suk Grace Kim.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, YS.G., Park, SH. Unpacking pathways using the direct and indirect effects model of writing (DIEW) and the contributions of higher order cognitive skills to writing. Read Writ 32, 1319–1343 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9913-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9913-y

Keywords

Navigation