Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 31, Issue 1, pp 1–33 | Cite as

Copying helps novice learners build orthographic knowledge: methods for teaching Devanagari akshara

  • Adeetee BhideEmail author
Article

Abstract

Hindi graphs, called akshara, are difficult to learn because of their visual complexity and large set of graphs. Akshara containing multiple consonants (complex akshara) are particularly difficult. In Hindi, complex akshara are formed by fusing individual consonantal graphs. Some complex akshara look similar to their component parts (transparent), whereas others do not (opaque). We taught 35 English-speaking adults a semi-artificial orthography that was modeled on the Devanagari script used for Hindi and other Indic languages. Participants were taught 80 complex akshara using 4 different methods: (1) choosing the components (from several choices) given the graph (2) choosing the correct graph (from several choices) given its components, (3) copying a graph while the graph and its components are displayed, and (4) writing a graph from memory given its components. Methods 1 and 2 compare emphasis on part-whole versus whole-part relationships, methods 1 & 2 and 3 & 4 compare motor effects, and methods 3 and 4 compare testing effects. We found that transparent graphs were better learned than opaque graphs. Testing on the akshara typically did not improve learning and there were few effects of emphasis on part-whole versus whole-part relationships. There was evidence for motor effects; copying & writing the akshara improved pure orthographic knowledge and people’s ability to produce the phonological form of a given akshara. These results corroborate other studies showing that copying and writing graphs helps beginning learners of English, Chinese, and Arabic build orthographic knowledge. Copying was more time efficient than writing, suggesting that having beginning learners copy akshara is an important pedagogical tool to use in classrooms.

Keywords

Orthographic learning Testing effect Handwriting Writing Multiple choice Akshara Alphasyllabary 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research partially fulfills the dissertation requirements for AB. I would like to thank my advisor, Charles Perfetti, for input on experimental design and feedback on manuscript drafts. I would also like to thank my committee members, Natasha Tokowicz, Julie Fiez, and Marta Ortega-Llebaria for help with experimental design and useful comments. Furthermore, I would like to thank Scott Fraundorf for his assistance with the statistical analyses. Moreover, I would like to thank my lab manager, Kimberly Muth, for assistance with IRB protocols and participant payment. Finally, I would like to thank Austin Marcus for help with data collection and entry. This research was supported by NSF PSLC [grant SBE08-36012] and the Andrew Mellon Predoctoral Fellowship awarded to AB.

References

  1. Aksharit. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.revolvy.com/main/index.php?s=Aksharit.
  2. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bhide, A., Gadgil, S., Zelinsky, C. M., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). Does reading in an alphasyllabary affect phonemic awareness? Inherent schwa effects in Marathi–English bilinguals. Writing Systems Research, 6(1), 73–93. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2013.855619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Butler, A. C., Marsh, E. J., Goode, M. K., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). When additional multiple-choice lures aid versus hinder later memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(7), 941–956. doi: 10.1002/acp.1239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (2006, May). Feedback neutralizes the detrimental effects of multiple-choice testing. Poster presented at the 18th annual convention for the association for psychological science, New York, NY.Google Scholar
  6. Cao, F., Vu, M., Chan, D. H. L., Lawrence, J. M., Harris, L. N., Guan, Q., et al. (2013). Writing affects the brain network of reading in Chinese: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping, 34(7), 1670–1684. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chang, L.-Y., Xu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, J., & Chen, H.-C. (2014). Supporting orthographic learning at the beginning stage of learning to read Chinese as a second language. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 61(3), 288–305. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2014.934016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Early spelling acquisition: Writing beats the computer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 159–162. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ehri, L. C., Gibbs, A. L., & Underwood, T. L. (1988). Influence of errors on learning the spellings of English words. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13(3), 236–253. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(88)90024-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Guan, C. Q., Liu, Y., Chan, D. H. L., Ye, F., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011). Writing strengthens orthography and alphabetic-coding strengthens phonology in learning to read Chinese. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 509–522. doi: 10.1037/a0023730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. James, K. H. (2010). Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. Developmental Science, 13(2), 279–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00883.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. James, K. H., & Atwood, T. P. (2009). The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: Tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(1), 91–110. doi: 10.1080/02643290802425914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. James, K. H., & Gauthier, I. (2006). Letter processing automatically recruits a sensory-motor brain network. Neuropsychologia, 44(14), 2937–2949. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Joshi, R. M. (2013). Literacy in Kannada, an alphasyllabic orthography. In H. Winskel & P. Padakannaya (Eds.), South and Southeast Asian psycholinguistics (pp. 184–191). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Longcamp, M., Boucard, C., Gilhodes, J.-C., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., Nazarian, B., et al. (2008). Learning through hand- or typewriting influences visual recognition of new graphic shapes: Behavioral and functional imaging evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 802–815. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.-T., & Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: A comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119(1), 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Martin-Chang, S., Ouellette, G., & Madden, M. (2014). Does poor spelling equate to slow reading? The relationship between reading, spelling, and orthographic quality. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(8), 1485–1505. doi: 10.1007/s11145-014-9502-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McDaniel, M. A., & Butler, A. C. (2011). A contextual framework for understanding when difficulties are desirable. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A festschrift in honor of Robert A Bjork (pp. 175–198)., New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  19. Nag, S. (2007). Early reading in Kannada: The pace of acquisition of orthographic knowledge and phonemic awareness. Journal of Research in Reading, 30(1), 7–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00329.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Nag, S. (2012). Multiple pathways to literacy: Findings from two longitudinal studies. In: International symposium on language, literacy and cognitive development. Bangalore: The Promise Foundation and University of York.Google Scholar
  21. Nag, S. (2014). Akshara-phonology mappings: The common yet uncommon case of the consonant cluster. Writing Systems Research, 6(1), 105–119. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2013.855621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nag, S., & Sircar, S. (2008). Learning to read in Bengali: Report of a survey in five Kolkata primary schools. Bangalore, India: The Promise Foundation.Google Scholar
  23. Nag, S., & Snowling, M. J. (2010). Cognitive profiles of poor readers of Kannada. Reading and Writing, 24(6), 657–676. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9258-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nag, S., Treiman, R., & Snowling, M. J. (2010). Learning to spell in an alphasyllabary: The case of Kannada. Writing Systems Research, 2(1), 41–52. doi: 10.1093/wsr/wsq001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Naka, M. (1998). Repeated writing facilitates children’s memory for pseudocharacters and foreign letters. Memory and Cognition, 26(4), 804–809. doi: 10.3758/BF03211399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Naka, M., & Naoi, H. (1995). The effect of repeated writing on memory. Memory and Cognition, 23(1), 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ouellette, G. (2010). Orthographic learning in learning to spell: The roles of semantics and type of practice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(1), 50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ouellette, G., & Tims, T. (2014). The write way to spell: Printing vs. typing effects on orthographic learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pandey, P. (2014). Akshara-to-sound rules for Hindi. Writing Systems Research, 6(1), 54–72. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2013.855622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Patel, P. G. (2004). Reading acquisition in India: Models of learning and dyslexia. New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  31. Patel, J., Bapi, R. S., & Nag, S. (2013). Akshara counts in child directed print: A pilot study with 101 texts. (Manuscript in Preparation).Google Scholar
  32. Rathod, P. H., Dhore, M. L., & Dhore, R. M. (2013). Hindi and Marathi to English machine transliteration using SVM. International Journal on Natural Language Computing, 2(4), 55–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rieth, H., Axelrod, S., Anderson, R., Hathaway, F., Wood, K., & Fitzgerald, C. (1974). Influence of distributed practice and daily testing on weekly spelling tests. The Journal of Educational Research, 68(2), 73–77. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1974.10884711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 181–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Share, D. L., & Daniels, P. T. (2016). Aksharas, alphasyllabaries, abugidas, alphabets and orthographic depth: Reflections on Rimzhim, Katz and Fowler (2014). Writing Systems Research, 8(1), 17–31. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2015.1016395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sinha, R. M. K., & Mahabala, H. N. (1979). Machine recognition of Devanagari script. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 9(8), 435–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tiwari, S. (2011). A preliminary investigation of akshara knowledge in the Malayalam alphasyllabary: Extension of Nag’s (2007) study. Writing Systems Research, 3(2), 145–151. doi: 10.1093/wsr/wsr013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Vasanta, D. (2004). Processing phonological information in a semi-syllabic script: Developmental data from Telugu. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17(1), 59–78. doi: 10.1023/B:READ.0000013830.55257.3a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., & Gordon, J. (1992). Early spelling acquisition: Does writing really beat the computer? Learning Disability Quarterly, 15(3), 223–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., & Gordon, J. (1993). Which motoric condition is most effective for teaching spelling to students with and without learning disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(3), 191–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wollscheid, S., Sjaastad, J., & Tømte, C. (2016). The impact of digital devices vs. Pen(cil) and paper on primary school students’ writing skills---A research review. Computers & Education, 95, 19–35. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2015.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Xu, Y., Chang, L. Y., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). The effect of radical-based grouping in character learning in Chinese as a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 98(3), 773–793. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2014.12122.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Xu, Y., Chang, L.-Y., Zhang, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2013). Reading, writing, and animation in character learning in Chinese as a foreign language. Foreign Language Annals, 46(3), 423–444. doi: 10.1111/flan.12040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of PittsburghPittsburghUSA
  2. 2.National University of SingaporeSingaporeSingapore

Personalised recommendations