Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of language specific factors on early written composition: the role of spelling, oral language and text generation skills in a shallow orthography

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Spelling skills have been identified as one of the major barriers to written text production in young English writers. By contrast oral language skills and text generation have been found to be less influential in the texts produced by beginning writers. To date, our understanding of the role of spelling skills in transparent orthographies is limited. The current study addressed this gap by examining the contribution of spelling, oral language and text generation skills in written text production in Italian beginner writers. Eighty-three children aged 7–8 years participated in the study. Spelling, lexical retrieval, receptive grammar, and written sentence generation and reformulation skills were assessed and children were asked to write a text on a set topic. A factor analysis revealed that the children’s written text production was captured by three factors: productivity, complexity and accuracy. In contrast to results from children learning to write in opaque orthographies, such as English, this study showed that receptive grammar and written sentence generation skills accounted for significant variance in measures of productivity, complexity and accuracy in Italian children’s written text production. Spelling skills contributed to text accuracy and quality and explained more variance than receptive grammar in microstructural accuracy. By contrast, oral grammatical skills explained more variance in text quality than spelling. The current study shows the differential impact of language systems, such as Italian, on written text production. Implications for assessment and instruction are outlined.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary-grade and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 478–508. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbott, R. D., Berninger, V. W., & Fayol, M. (2010). Longitudinal relationships of levels of language in writing and between writing and reading in grades 1 to 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 281–298. doi:10.1037/a0019318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arfé, B., De Bernardi, B., Pasini, M., & Poeta, F. (2012). Toward a redefinition of spelling in shallow orthographies: Phonological, lexical and grammatical skills in learning to spell Italian. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), Past, present, and future contributions of cognitive writing research to cognitive psychology (pp. 359–387). London, NY: Psychology Press, Taylor Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arfé, B., Dockrell, J. E., & Berninger, V. W. (2014). Writing development in children with hearing loss, dyslexia or oral language problems: Implications for assessment and instruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arfé, B., & Pizzocaro, E. (2016). Sentence generation in children with and without problems of written expression. In J. Perera, M. Aparici, E. Rosado, & N. Salas (Eds.), Written and spoken language development across the lifespan Essays in honor of Liliana Tolchinsky (pp. 327–344). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Babayigit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2010). Component processes of early reading, spelling, and narrative writing skills in Turkish: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 23(5), 539–568. doi:10.1007/S11145-009-9173-Y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babayigit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2011). Modeling the relationships between cognitive-linguistic skills and literacy skills: New insights from a transparent orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 169–189. doi:10.1037/a0021671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbiero, C., Lonciari, I., Montico, M., Monasta, L., Penge, R., et al. (2012). The Submerged dyslexia iceberg: How many school children are not diagnosed? Results from an Italian study. PLoS ONE, 7(10), 1–9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. (2014). Linguistic perspectives on writing development. In B. Arfé, J. Dockrell, & V. Berninger (Eds.), Writing development in children with hearing loss, dyslexia or oral language problems: Implications for assessment and instruction (pp. 176–186). NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W. (2000). Development of language by hand and its connections with language by ear, mouth, and eye. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(4), 65–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Nagy, W., & Beers, S. (2011). Child writers’ construction and reconstruction of single sentences and construction of multi-sentence texts: Contributions of syntax and transcription to translation. Reading and Writing, 24(2), 151–182. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9262-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flowers’ model of skilled writing to explain developing writing. In E. C. Butterfield (Ed.), Advances in cognition and educational practice. Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing (Vol. 2, pp. 1–30). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R. D., Begay, K., Coleman, K. B., Curtin, G., & Graham, S. (2002). Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: Implications for the simple view of writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 291–304. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.94.2.291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Graham, S., Abbott, R. D., Brooks, A., Abbott, S. P., et al. (1998). Early intervention for spelling problems: Teaching functional spelling units of varying size with a multiple-connections framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(4), 587–605. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.4.587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower -level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing, 4(3), 257–280. doi:10.1007/bf01027151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1982). Test for the reception of grammar. Oxford, UK: Medical Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiacchi, P., Cendron, M., Gugliotta, M., Tressoldi, P., & Vio, C. (2005). BVN 5-11: Batteria di valutazione neuropsicologica per l’età evolutiva [Battery for neuropsychological assessment in children]. Trento: Erikson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brizzolara, D. (1989). Test di vocabolario figurato [Picture naming test. Technical report of the research project 500.4/62.1/1134 supported by a grant from the Italian Department of Health]. Pisa: IRCCS Stella Maris.

  • Broc, L., Bernicot, J., Olive, T., Favart, M., Reilly, J., Quemart, P., & Uze, J. (2013). Lexical spelling in children and adolescents with specific language impairment: Variations with the writing situation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 3253–3266. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in Medicine, 28, 205–220. doi:10.1002/sim.3471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, G., & Finch, S. (2005). Inference by eye: Confidence intervals, and how to read pictures of data. American Psychologist, 60, 170–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dall’Oglio, A. M., Rossiello, B., Coletti, M. F., Bultrini, M., de Marchis, C., Rava, L., & Cuttini, M. (2010). Do healthy preterm children need neuropsychological follow-up? Preschool outcomes compared with term peers. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 52(10), 955–961. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03730.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danzak, R. L., & Arfé, B. (2016). Globally minded text production: Bilingual, expository writing of Italian adolescents learning English. Topics in Language Disorders, 36(1).

  • Dockrell, J. E., & Connelly, V. (2015). The role of oral language in underpinning the text generation difficulties in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(1), 18–34. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01550.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dockrell, J., Marshall, C., & Wyse, D. (2015). Teachers’ reported practices for teaching writing in England. Reading and Writing, Early View. doi:10.1007/s11145-015-9605-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Englert, C. S., Raphael, T. E., Anderson, L. M., Anthony, H. M., & Stevens, D. D. (1991). Making strategies and self-talk visible—writing instruction in regular and special-education classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 28(2), 337–372. doi:10.3102/00028312028002337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students. A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 170–182. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., McKeown, D., Kiuhara, S., & Harris, K. R. (2012). A Meta-Analysis of Writing Instruction for Students in the Elementary Grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 879–896. doi:10.1037/a0029185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1986). Writing research and the writer. American Psychologist, 41(10), 1106–1113. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.41.10.1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, K. W. (1965). Grammatical structure written at three grade levels. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, Research Report No. 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write—a longitudinal-study of 54 children from 1st through 4th grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437–447. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, S., Wanzek, J., Petscher, Y., Al Otaiba, S., & Kim, Y. S. (2014). Writing fluency and quality in kindergarten and first grade: the role of attention, reading, transcription, and oral language. Reading and Writing, 27(7), 1163–1188. doi:10.1007/s11145-013-9480-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Folsom, J. S., Gruelich, L., & Puranik, C. (2014). Evaluating the dimensionality of first grade writing composition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 199–211. doi:10.1044/1092-4388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Puranik, C., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, L., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Componential skills of beginning writing: An exploratory study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 517–525. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. S., Al Otaiba, S., Wanzek, J., & Gatlin, B. (2015). Toward an understanding of dimensions, predictors, and the gender gap in written composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 79–95. doi:10.1037/a0037210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limpo, T., & Alves, R. A. (2013). Modeling writing development: Contribution of transcription and self-regulation to portuguese students’ text generation quality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 401–413. doi:10.1037/a0031391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorusso, M. L., Vernice, M., Dieterich, M., Brizzolara, D., Mariani, E., Masi, S. D., & Mele, A. (2014). The process and criteria for diagnosing specific learning disorders: Indications from the Consensus Conference promoted by the Italian National Institute of Health. Annali dell’Istituto Superiore di Sanità, 50(1), 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, C. J., Dockrell, J., & Lindsay, G. (2013). An evaluation of the written texts of children with SLI: The contributions of oral language, reading and phonological short-term memory. Reading and Writing, 26(6), 865–888. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9396-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, H. S., Voeten, M. J. M., Vauras, M. M. S., & Poskiparta, E. H. (2001). Predicting writing skill development with word recognition and preschool readiness skills. Reading and Writing, 14(7–8), 643–672. doi:10.1023/a:1012071514719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R. J. (2001). Assessment of language disorders in children. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D., Covill, A., Hoyne, S. H., & Mildes, K. (1994). Individual differences in writing—implications of translating fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 256–266. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.86.2.256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Assessment of Educational Progress. (2007). The nation’s report card. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://nationsreportcard.gov/writing_2007/w0001.asp.

  • Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(1), 1–17. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinto, G., Tarchi, C., & Bigozzi, L. (2015). The relationship between oral and written narratives: A three-year longitudinal study of narrative cohesion, coherence, and structure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(4), 551–569. doi:10.1111/bjep.12091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puranik, C. S., Lombardino, L. J., & Altmann, L. J. P. (2008). Assessing the microstructure of written language using a retelling paradigm. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(2), 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I. (2003). Lexicality effects in single-word naming in alphabetic Turkish orthography. In M. Joshi, C. K. Leong, & B. L. J. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Literacy acquisition: Role of phonology, morphology and orthography (pp. 83–93). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS Press of Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J. S., Bernicot, J., Olive, T., Uzé, J., Wulfeck, B., Favart, M., & Appelbaum, M. (2014). Written narratives from French and Egnlish speaking children with lagnuage impairment. In B. Arfé, J. Dockrell, & V. Berninger (Eds.), Writing development in children with hearing loss, dyslexia or oral language problems: Implications for assessment and instruction (pp. 176–186). Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Cipolotti, L. (1998). Dynamic aphasia: An inability to select between competing verbal responses? Brain, 121, 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rustioni Metz-Lancaster, D., & Associazione “La nostra famiglia”. (1994). Prove di valutazione della comprensione linguistica [Test for the assessment of linguistic comprehension]. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., & Tressoldi, P. (2007). DDE-2. Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva [Battery for the assessment of developmental dyslexia and dysorthographia]. Firenze: Giunti OS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, M., Alamargot, D., Castelló, M., Ganier, F., Kruse, O., Mangen, A., & van Waes, L. (2012). Learning to write effectively: Current trends in European research. Bingley: Emerald.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tressoldi, P. E., & Cornoldi, C. (1991). Batteria per la valutazione della scrittura e della competenza ortografica nella scuola dell’obbligo [BVSCO, Battery for the assessment of writing skills of children from 7 to 13 years old]. Firenze, Italy: Giunti OS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicari, S., Menghini, D., Di Paola, M., Serra, L., Donfrancesco, A., Fidani, P., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2007). Acquired amnesia in childhood: A single case study. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 704–715. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Puranik, C. S., Foorman, B., Foster, E., Wilson, L. G., Tschinkel, E., & Kantor, P. T. (2011). Modeling the development of written language. Reading and Writing, 24(2), 203–220. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9266-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables. Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 56–75). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by MIUR—PRIN 2006 Grant Nos. 111929 and ISCH COST Action IS1401. The authors wish to thank Magali Boureux and Francesca Poeta for their help in scoring the texts having coded the texts for inter-rater agreement.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara Arfé.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Arfé, B., Dockrell, J.E. & De Bernardi, B. The effect of language specific factors on early written composition: the role of spelling, oral language and text generation skills in a shallow orthography. Read Writ 29, 501–527 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9617-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9617-5

Keywords

Navigation