Skip to main content
Log in

The double deficit hypothesis in the transparent Finnish orthography: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 2

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined the double deficit hypothesis (Wolf & Bowers, 1999) and literacy development in a longitudinal dataset of 1,006 Finnish children who were nonreaders at school entry. A single phonological awareness (PA) deficit was a predictor of pseudoword spelling accuracy and reading fluency, and a single rapid automatized naming (RAN) deficit was a predictor of reading fluency. The group with both PA and RAN deficits experienced the most extensive reading and spelling difficulties. However, all groups included both poor and average Grade 2 readers and spellers. Poor letter knowledge and vocabulary, task avoidance, attention difficulties, hyperactivity, and lack of teaching at home were additional risk factors for reading and spelling problems, but their impact varied depending on the presence of PA and RAN deficits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Review and Research, 11, 253–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M. (2006). Learning to read: The effect of orthography. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 531–550). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aro, T., Semrud-Clikeman, E., Lapveteläinen, A.-M., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Developmental underpinnings of the association of ADHD and its subtypes to neuropsychological and academic weaknesses. In D. Gonzal & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: From genes to patients (pp. 293–316). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2007). Preliterate phonological awareness and early literacy skills in Turkish. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 394–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., Genesee, F., & Caravolas, M. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 145–162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Bruck, M., & Genesee, F. (2003). Similarities and differences between English- and French-speaking poor spellers. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages (pp. 157–180). London, UK: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Vólin, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Davis, C. J., DeFries, J. C., Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and RAN: An overview of results from the Colorado twin study. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Conference proceedings of the dyslexia research foundation conference in extraordinary brain series: Time, fluency, and developmental dyslexia (pp. 277–303). Baltimore, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, C. B., Harach, T. W., Cortes, R. C., Abbott, R. D., & Catalano, R. F. (2004). Level and change in reading scores and attention problems during elementary school as predictors of problem behavior in middle school. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 12, 130–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme sensitivity. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 97–118). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, A., Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 203–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency in English and Greek: A cross-linguistic comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Manolitsis, G., Nurmi, J.-E., & Parrila, R. (2010). Does task-focused versus task-avoidance behavior matter for literacy development in an orthographically consistent language? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 321–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Germanò, E., Gagliano, A., & Curatolo, P. (2010). Comorbidity of ADHD and Dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 475–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. L., Wood, F. B., Meyer, M. S., Hart, L. A., Speed, W. C., Shuster, A., et al. (1997). Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15. American Journal of Human Genetics, 60, 27–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halonen, A., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2006). The role of learning to read in the development of problem behaviour: A cross-lagged longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 517–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of children’s school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72, 625–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirvonen, R., Georgiou, G. K., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2010). Task-focused behaviour and literacy development: A reciprocal relationship. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 302–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y.-S., Wolf, M., Morris, R., & Lovett, M. W. (2008). The varieties of pathways to dysfluent reading: Comparing subtypes of children with dyslexia at letter, word, and connected text levels of reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 47–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J., Georgiou, G., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korkman, M., Barron-Linnankoski, S., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (1999). Effects of age and duration of reading instruction on the development of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 415–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kortteinen, H., Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (2009). Does IQ matter in adolescents’ reading disability? Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 257–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word recognition in Grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 367–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leppänen, U., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2004). Development of reading skills among preschool and primary school pupils. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 72–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leppänen, U., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2006). Development of reading and spelling Finnish from preschool to Grade 1 and Grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 3–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K., Niemi, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Poskiparta, M., Siekkinen, M., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2006a). The ongoing first steps study. Finland: Universities of Jyväskylä, Joensuu, and Turku.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Ketonen, R. (2006). ARMI—Luku-ja kirjoitustaidon arviointimateriaali 1. Luokalle [ARMI—A tool for assessing reading and writing skills in Grade 1]. Helsinki, Finland: WSOY.

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2004). Predicting reading performance during the first and the second year of primary school. British Educational Research Journal, 30, 67–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the development of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 1040–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindeman, J. (1998). ALLUAla-asteen lukutesti [ALLU—reading test for primary school]. Finland: University of Turku, Turku, Finland, The Center for Learning Research.

  • Logan, S., & Johnson, R. (2009). Gender differences in reading ability and attitudes: Examining where those differences lie. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W., Steinbach, K. A., & Frijters, J. C. (2000). Remediating the core deficits of developmental reading disability: A double-deficit perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 334–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., & Guttorm, T. (2008). Early identification and prevention of dyslexia: Results from a prospective follow-up study of children at familial risk for dyslexia. In G. Reid, A. Fawcett, F. Manis, & L. Siegel (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of dyslexia (pp. 121–146). London: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., Stephenson, K., & Parrila, R. (2009). Beginning to read across language varying in orthographic consistency: Comparing the effects of cognitive and non-cognitive predictors. Learning and Instruction, 19, 466–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic sensitivity and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morfidi, E., van der Leij, A., de Jong, P., Scheltinga, F., & Bekebrede, J. (2007). Reading in two orthographies: A cross-linguistic study of Dutch average and poor readers who learn English as a second language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 753–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. (1998–2010). Mplus user’s guide. Version 6. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2000). The role of task-avoidant and task-focussed behaviors in the development of reading and mathematical skills during the first school year: A cross-lagged longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 478–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öney, B., & Durgunoglu, A. Y. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G., & Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and the double deficit hypotheses of developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petrill, S. A., Hart, S. A., Harlaar, N., Logan, J., Justice, L. M., & Schatschneider, C. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on the growth of early reading skills. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 660–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. M., Norris, S. P., Osmond, W. C., & Maynard, A. M. (2002). Relative reading achievement: A longitudinal study of 187 children from first through sixth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 3–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Poikkeus, A.-M. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 923–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. P., & Lubienski, S. T. (2011). The development of gender achievement gaps in mathematics and reading during elementary and middle school: Examining direct cognitive assessments and teacher ratings. American Educational Research Journal, 48, 268–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Caspi, A., Fergusson, D., Horwood, L. J., Goodman, R., Maughan, B., et al. (2004). Sex differences in developmental reading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. Journal of American Medical Association, 291, 2007–2012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelsson, S., Olson, R., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., Defries, J. C., & Wilcutt, E. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on prereading skills and early reading and spelling development in the United States, Australia, and Scandinavia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 51–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 1, pp. 97–110). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B., & Fletcher, J. (2002). Relationships of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvén, M., Poskiparta, E., & Niemi, P. (2004). The odds of becoming a precocious reader of Finnish. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 152–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, K., Parrila, R., Georgiou, G., & Kirby, J. (2008). Effects of home literacy, parents’ beliefs, and children’s task-focused behavior on emergent literacy and word reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 24–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sunseth, K., & Bowers, P. G. (2002). Rapid naming and phonemic awareness: Contributions to reading, spelling, and orthographic knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 401–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: Meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of Educational Research, 73, 407–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15, 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Georgiou, G., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2012). Examining the double-deficit hypothesis in an orthographically consistent language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 287–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Difficulties, 43, 308–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. V., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What we have learned in the past four decades. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N. A., & Hulslander, J. L. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, X., Nurmi, J.-E., Kiuru, N., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Aunola, K. (2011). A teacher-report measure of children’s task-avoidant behavior: A validation study of the Behavioral Strategy Rating Scale. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 690–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by a grant from the Academy of Finland for the Finnish Centre of Excellence in Learning and Motivation Research (#213486 and #126043) and other grant from the same funding agency to Minna Torppa (#2106311). Funding sources have had no role in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Minna Torppa.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Torppa, M., Parrila, R., Niemi, P. et al. The double deficit hypothesis in the transparent Finnish orthography: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 2. Read Writ 26, 1353–1380 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9423-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9423-2

Keywords

Navigation