Skip to main content

Vowel representations in the invented spellings of Spanish–English bilingual kindergartners

Abstract

The study compared the invented spelling of vowels in kindergarten native Spanish speaking children with that of English monolinguals. It examined whether, after receiving phonics instruction for short vowels, the spelling of native Spanish-speaking kindergartners would contain phonological errors that were influenced by their first language. Results showed no differences between the two groups on the number of correct short vowel spellings, even though the sounds for four of the five English short vowels do not exist in Spanish. By contrast, differences were observed in the representation of long vowels with a higher rate of error among ELLs. The students had not received explicit instruction in long vowels. ELLs appeared to be trying to represent the diphthongized nature of some English long vowels by spelling long vowels with more than one vowel. The results support the authors’ hypothesis that kindergarten phonics instruction had an impact on the invented spellings of new second language vowel phonemes.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References

  • Adrián, J., Alegria, J., & Morais, J. (1995). Metaphonological abilities of Spanish illiterate adults. International Journal of Psychology, 30, 329–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (2005). Plasticity, localization and language development. In S. Parker, J. Langer, & C. Milbrath (Eds.), Biology & knowledge revisited: From neurogenesis to psychogenesis (pp. 205–236). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bear, D., Templeton, S., Helman, L., & Baren, T. (2003). Orthographic development and learning to read in different languages. In G. Garcia (Ed.), English learners: Reaching the highest level of English literacy (pp. 71–95). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language literacy and cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (2000). Vowel categorization skill and its relationship to early literacy skills among first-grade Québec-French children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 190–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas-Hagan, E., Carlson, C., & Pollard-Durodola, S. (2007). The cross-linguistic transfer of early literacy skills: The role of initial L1 and L2 skills and language of instruction. Language Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 38, 249–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Chen, H.-C., Lay, C., Wong, O., & Hills, M. (2001). The development of phonological awareness: Effects of spoken language experience and orthography. Cognition, 81, 227–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Glaeser, B., & Ferko, D. (2007). Speech perception, vocabulary, and the development of reading skills in English among Korean- and English-speaking children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 154–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciscero, C., & Royer, J. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 275–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L. E., & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., Clark-Chiarelli, N., & Wolfe, A. (2004). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 323–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgonoglu, A., Nagy, W., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1984). How orthography alters spoken language competencies in children learning to read and spell. In J. Downing & R. Valtin (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 119–148). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1993). How English orthography influences phonological knowledge as children learn to read and spell. In R. Scholes (Ed.), Literacy and language analysis (pp. 21–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (2000). Learning to read and learning to spell: Two sides of a coin. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 19–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fashola, O., Drum, P., Mayer, R., & Kang, S. (1996). A cognitive theory of orthographic transitioning: Predictable errors in how Spanish-speaking children spell English words. American Educational Research Journal, 33, 825–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferroli, L. (1991). Developmental spelling and transfer of literacy skills among primary grade bilinguals. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED362010).

  • Gentry, J. R. (2000). A retrospective on invented spelling and a look forward. The Reading Teacher, 54, 318–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, B., & Kester, E. (2004). Spanish-influenced English-typical phonological patterns in the English language learner. Bilinguistics. Retrieved from http://www.pediastaff.com/uploads/resources/abad_0701.pdf.

  • Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia, 6, 133–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, R. (2001). The sounds of Spanish: Analysis and application. Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Kroll, J. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 2–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., Relkin, N., Lee, L., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second-languages. Nature, 388, 171–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N., & Siegel, L. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1005–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, L., & Greenfield, D. (2004). The cross-language transfer of phonological skills of Hispanic Head Start children. Bilingual Research Journal, 28, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marian, V., Spivey, M., & Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language, 86, 70–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1998). The development of invented spelling. Early Education and Development, 9, 147–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meschyan, G., & Hernandez, A. (2004). Cognitive factors in second-language acquisition and literacy learning: A theoretical proposal and call for research. In C. A. Stone, E. Silliman, B. Ehren, & K. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 73–81). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J., & Walley, A. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. C. (2005/2006). How spelling supports reading: And why it is more regular and predictable than you think. American Educator, 29, 12–22, 42–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. (1999). Case studies in teaching beginning readers: The Howard street tutoring manual. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarra, J., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2005). The perception of second-language sounds in early bilinguals: New evidence from an implicit measure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 912–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., Paulesu, E., Brambati, S., Scifo, P., Cappa, S., et al. (2003). The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high-proficient bilinguals: An fMRI study during verbal fluency. Human Brain Mapping, 19, 170–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., Beck, I., Bell, L., & Hughes, C. (1988). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. In K. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 39–76). Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raynolds, L. B. (2007). Differential acquisition of English letter-sound correspondences in bilingual and monolingual primary students. ETD collection for Fordham University. Paper AAI3262837. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/304873938.

  • Raynolds, L. B., & Uhry, J. K. (2010). The invented spellings of non-Spanish phonemes by Spanish–English bilingual and English monolingual kindergarteners. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 495–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read, C. (1971). Preschool children’s knowledge of English orthography. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, C. (1986). Children’s creative spelling. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richgels, D. (2001). Invented spelling, phonemic awareness, and reading and writing instruction. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 142–155). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • San Francisco, A. R., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2006a). The role of language of instruction and vocabulary in the English phonological awareness of Spanish–English bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(2), 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • San Francisco, A. R., Mo, E., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2006b). The influences of language of literacy instruction and vocabulary on the spelling of Spanish–English bilinguals. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 627–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, S., & Morris, D. (2000). Spelling. In M. L. Kamil, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 525–543). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uhry, J. (1999). Invented spelling in kindergarten: The relationship with fingerpoint reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 441–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhry, J. K., & Goodman, N. E. (2009). Teacher linguistic knowledge and student literacy outcomes: Putting research into practice. In S. Rosenfield & V. Berninger (Eds.), Implementing evidence-based interventions in school settings. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. (2007). Early bilingualism, language transfer, and phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 425–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., & Geva, E. (2003). Spelling acquisition of novel English phonemes in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 325–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, B. A. (2002). Teacher’s manual, Levels K-1 (Fundations). Millbury, MA: Wilson Language Training.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yavas, M. (2006). Applied English phonology. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeong, S., & Rickard Liow, S. (2010). Phonemic representation and early spelling errors in bilingual children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 387–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zutell, J. (1988). The English spelling strategies of Spanish-speaking bilingual children. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 333–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura B. Raynolds.

Appendix: Spelling measure

Appendix: Spelling measure

 

 

1. Thick

15. Back

2. Cute

16. Feet

3. Shine

17. Step

4. Ball

18. Junk

5. Then

19. Picking

6. Rushed

20. Mail

7. Took

21. Side

8. Shopping

22. Chin

9. Saw

23. Dress

10. Tooth

24. Peeked

11. Brave

25. Lamp

12. Bug

26. Road

13. Vote

 

14. Job

 

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Raynolds, L.B., Uhry, J.K. & Brunner, J. Vowel representations in the invented spellings of Spanish–English bilingual kindergartners. Read Writ 26, 647–664 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9380-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9380-9

Keywords