Abstract
Alternative models of the structure of individual and developmental differences of written composition and handwriting fluency were tested using confirmatory factor analysis of writing samples provided by first- and fourth-grade students. For both groups, a five-factor model provided the best fit to the data. Four of the factors represented aspects of written composition: macro-organization (use of top sentence and number and ordering of ideas), productivity (number and diversity of words used), complexity (mean length of T-unit and syntactic density), and spelling and punctuation. The fifth factor represented handwriting fluency. Handwriting fluency was correlated with written composition factors at both grades. The magnitude of developmental differences between first grade and fourth grade expressed as effect sizes varied for variables representing the five constructs: large effect sizes were found for productivity and handwriting fluency variables; moderate effect sizes were found for complexity and macro-organization variables; and minimal effect sizes were found for spelling and punctuation variables.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Notes
Our approach to developmental comparisons may seem somewhat minimalist. For mean differences, we simply reported effect sizes and the results of multiple t-tests, noting that a Bonferroni adjustment did not result in a significant difference becoming nonsignificant. We took this approach for several reasons. First, our primary interest was in the magnitudes of the effect sizes. Finding significant differences in the mean performance of first and fourth grade students on language measures would not be ground breaking. Similarly, given our modest sample sizes, we merely reported the results of separate confirmatory factor analyses of the first- and fourth-grade samples, focusing on the overall pattern of results as opposed to the results of a detailed, multi-group analysis of significant differences in parameter estimates that may not generalize.
References
Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary- and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478–508.
Alves, R. A, Castro, S. L., Sousa, L., & Stromqvist, S. (2007). Influence of typing skill on pause-execution cycles in written composition. In M. Torrance, L. van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 55–65).
Bazerman, C. (2008). Handbook of research on writing: History society, school, individual, text. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Berman, R., & Verhoevan, L. (2002). Cross-linguistic perspectives on the development of text-production abilities. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 1–43.
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Whitaker, D., Sylvester, L., & Nolen, S. B. (1995). Integrating low- and high-level skills in instructional protocols for writing disabilities. Learning Disabilies Quarterly, 18, 293–309.
Berninger, V. W., & Chanquoy, L. (in press). Writing development: What writing is and how it changes over early and middle childhood. In E. Grigorenko, E. Mambrino, & D. Priess (Eds.), Handbook of writing: A Mosaic of perspectives and views. New York: Psychology Press.
Berninger, V., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of development of skilled writing (pp. 57–81). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R., Begay, K., Byrd, K., & Curtin, G. (2002). Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: Implications for the simple view of writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 291–304.
Berninger, V. W., & Winn, W. (2006). Implications of advancements in brain research and technology for writing development, writing instruction, and education evolution. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 96–114). New York: Guilford.
Chanquoy, L., & Alamargot, D. (2002). Working memory and writing: Evolution of models and assessment of research. Annee Psychologique, 102, 363–398.
Christensen, C. A. (2005). The role of orthographic-motor integration in the production of creative and well-structured written text for students in secondary school. Educational Psychology, 25, 441–453.
Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower-order letter and work fluency skills to written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 175–198.
Connelly, V., Dockrell, J., & Barnett, J. (2005). The slow handwriting of undergraduate students constrains overall performance in exam essays. Educational Psychology, 25, 99–107.
Dockrell, J., Lindsay, G., & Connelly, V. (2009). The impact of specific language impairment on adolescents’ written text. Exceptional Children, 75, 427–436.
Fayol, M. (1997). On acquiring and using punctuation: A study of written French. In J. Costermans & M. Fayol (Eds.), Processing interclausal relationships: Studies in the production and comprehension of text (pp. 157–178). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 31–50). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2005). Writing better: Effective strategies for teaching students with learning difficulties. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2009). Almost 30 years of writing research: Making sense of it all with The Wrath of Khan. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 24, 58–68.
Grigorenko, E., Mambrino, E., & Priess, D. (Eds.). (in press). Handbook of writing: A Mosaic of perspectives and views. New York: Psychology Press.
Harris, K. R., Graham, S., Mason, L., & Friedlander, B. (2008). Powerful writing strategies for all students. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 1–27). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hayes, J. R., & Flowers, L. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 3–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Houck, C., & Billingsley, B. (1989). Written expression of students with and without learning disabilities: Differences across the grades. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 561–565.
Hunt, K. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels (Research Report No. 3.). Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Huot, B., & Neal, M. (2006). Writing assessment. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 417–432). New York: Guilford.
Juel, C., Griffith, P., & Gough, P. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.
MacAuthur, C. A., Graham, S., & Fitzgerald, J. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of writing research. New York: Guilford Press.
Mackie, C., & Dockrell, J. (2004). The nature of written language deficits in children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1469–1483.
McCutchen, D. (2006). Cognitive factors in the development of children’s writing. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 115–130). New York: Guilford.
Mehta, P. D., Foorman, B. R., Branum-Martin, L., & Taylor, W. P. (2005). Literacy as a unidimensional multilevel construct: Validation, sources of influence, and implications in a longitudinal study in grades 1 to 4. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 85–116.
Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (2001). Systematic analysis of language transcripts (Version 7.0) [computer software]. Madison, WI: Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Moran, M. (1981). Performance of learning disabled and low achieving secondary students on formal features of a paragraph-writing task. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 271–280.
Nelson, N. W., Bahr, C., & Van Meter, A. (2004). The writing lab approach to language instruction and intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.
Nelson, N. W., & Van Meter, A. (2002). Assessing curriculum-based reading and writing samples. Topics in Language Disorders, 22, 35–59.
Nelson, N. W., & Van Meter, A. M. (2007). Measuring written language ability in narrative samples. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23, 287–309.
Nystrand, M. (2006). The social and historical context for writing research. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 11–27). New York: Guilford.
Olive, T., Alves, R. A., & Castro, S. L. (in press). Cognitive processes in writing during pauses and execution periods. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology.
Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2002). Concurrent activation of high- and low-level production processes in written composition. Memory & Cognition, 30, 594–600.
Peverly, S. T. (2006). The importance of handwriting speed in adult writing. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 197–216.
Puranik, C., Lombardino, L., & Altmann, L. (2007). Writing through retellings: An exploratory study of language impaired and dyslexic populations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 251–272.
Puranik, C., Lombardino, L., & Altmann, L. (2008). Assessing the microstructure of written language using a retelling paradigm. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 107–120.
Rice, J. M. (1897). The futility of the spelling grind. Forum, XXIII, 163–172.
Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W., & Fayol, M. (2009). fMRI activation differences between 11-year-old good and poor spellers’ access in working memory to temporary and long-term orthographic representations. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 327–353.
Sanders, T. J. M., & Schilperood, J. (2006). Text structure as a window on the cognition of writing: How text analysis provides insights in writing products and writing processes. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 386–402). New York: Guilford.
Scott, C., & Stokes, S. (1995). Measures of syntax in school-age children and adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 309–317.
Scott, C., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written discourse produced by school-age children with and without language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 324–339.
Shanahan, T. (1984). Nature of the reading-writing relation: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 466–477.
Shanahan, T. (1988). The reading-writing relationship: Seven instructional principles. Reading Teacher, 41, 636–647.
Shanahan, T. (2006). Relations among oral language, reading, and writing development. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 171–183). New York: Guilford.
Shanahan, T., & Lomax, R. G. (1986). An analysis and comparison of theoretical models of the reading–writing relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 116–123.
Torrance, M., & Galbraith, D. (2006). The processing demands of writing. In C. MacAuthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 67–80). New York: Guilford.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 1–20.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.
Whitaker, D., Berninger, V., Johnston, J., & Swanson, L. (1994). Intraindividual differences in levels of language in intermediate grade writers: Implications for the translating process. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 107–130.
Acknowledgments
Support for carrying out this research was provided by grant P50 HD052120 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and by Postdoctoral Training Grant R305B050032 and Predoctoral Interdisciplinary Research Training Grant R305B04074 from the Institute of Education Sciences.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wagner, R.K., Puranik, C.S., Foorman, B. et al. Modeling the development of written language. Read Writ 24, 203–220 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9266-7
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9266-7