Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2003). Effects of stress typicality during speeded grammatical classification. Language and Speech,
46, 353–374.
Article
Google Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2004). The effects of stress typicality during spoken word recognition by native and non-native speakers: Evidence from onset-gating. Memory and Cognition,
32, 21–30.
Google Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2006). The processing of lexical stress during visual word recognition: Typicality effects and orthographic correlates. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
59, 920–948.
Article
Google Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2007). Would you rather ‘embert a cudsert’ or ‘cudsert an embert’? How spelling patterns at the beginning of English disyllables can cue grammatical category. In A. Schalley & D. Khlentzos (Eds.), Mental states: Language and cognitive structure (pp. 213–237).
Arciuli, J., & Monaghan, P. (2006). Hidden cues to grammatical category in the spelling of English disyllables The 15th Australian Language and Speech Conference. Australian Journal of Psychology,
58, 1–13.
Google Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Slowiaczek, L. (2007). The where and when of linguistic word level prosody. Neuropsychologia,
45, 2638–2642.
Article
Google Scholar
Arévalo, A., Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Butler, A., Bates, E., & Dronkers, N. (2007). Action and object processing in aphasia: From nouns and verbs to the effect of manipulability. Brain and Language,
100, 79–94.
Article
Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.
Google Scholar
Baker, R. G., & Smith, P. T. (1976). A psycholinguistic study of English stress assignment rules. Language and Speech,
19, 9–17.
Google Scholar
Black, M., & Chiat, S. (2003). Noun-verb dissociations: A multi-faceted phenomenon. Journal of Neurolinguistics,
16, 231–250.
Article
Google Scholar
Boland, J. E. (1997). Resolving syntactic category ambiguities in discourse context: Probabilistic and discourse constraints. Journal of Memory and Language,
36, 588–615.
Article
Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Cassar, M., & Treiman, R. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children’s knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology,
89, 631–644.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., & Kelly, M. (1991). Phonological information for grammatical category assignments. Journal of Memory and Language,
30, 348–369.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., & Kelly, M. (2001). Children’s use of phonology to infer grammatical class in vocabulary learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Journal,
8, 519–523.
Google Scholar
Cazden, C. B. (1976). Play with language and metalinguistic awareness: One dimension of language experience. In J. S. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 603–608). New York: Basic Books.
Google Scholar
D’Amico, S., Bentrovato, S., Gasparini, M., Costabile, D., & Bates, E. (2002). Timed picture-naming in Italian-speaking children and adults: Differences between nouns and verbs. Paper Presented at IASCL-SRCLD, Madison, WI.
Davis, S. M., & Kelly, M. (1997). Knowledge of the English noun-verb stress difference by native and nonnative speakers. Journal of Memory and Language,
36, 445–460.
Article
Google Scholar
Durieux, G., & Gillis, S. (2001). Predicting grammatical classes from phonological cues: An empirical test. In J. Weissenborn & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to bootstrapping: Phonological, lexical, syntactic and neurophysiological aspects of early language acquisition (Vol. 1, pp. 189–229). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Farmer, T. A., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2006). Phonological typicality influences lexical processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
103, 12203–12208.
Article
Google Scholar
Fay, D., & Cutler, A. (1977). Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon. Linguistic Inquiry,
8, 505–520.
Google Scholar
Fudge, E. (1984). English word stress. London: Allen & Unwin.
Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H., Landau, B., & Wanner, E. (1988). Where learning begins: Initial representations for language learning. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey (Vol. 3, pp. 150–193). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. New York: Harvester.
Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1982). Why are nouns learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. Kuczaj II (Ed.), Language development, Vol. 2: Language, thought, and culture. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (2006). Why verbs are hard to learn. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp. 544–564). New York: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Hauerwas, L. B., & Walker, J. (2003). Spelling of inflected verb morphology in children with spelling deficits. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,
18, 25–35.
Article
Google Scholar
Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). Wide range achievement test-revised 3. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.
Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1987). Function and process in comparing language and cognition. In M. Hickmann (Ed.), Social and functional approaches to language and thought (pp. 185–202). New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kelly, M. H. (1988). Phonological biases in grammatical category shifts. Journal of Memory and Language,
27, 343–358.
Article
Google Scholar
Kelly, M. H. (1992). Using sound to solve syntactic problems: The role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. Psychological Review,
99, 349–364.
Article
Google Scholar
Kelly, M. H., & Bock, J. K. (1988). Stress in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
14, 389–403.
Article
Google Scholar
Kelly, M. H., Morris, J., & Verrekia, L. (1998). Orthographic cues to lexical stress: Effects on naming and lexical decision. Memory and Cognition,
26, 822–832.
Google Scholar
Kemp, N., & Bryant, P. (2003). Do beez buzz? Rule-based and frequency-based knowledge in learning to spell plural -s. Child Development,
74, 63–74.
Article
Google Scholar
Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2003). Is English spelling chaotic? Misconceptions concerning its irregularity. Reading Psychology,
24, 267–289.
Article
Google Scholar
Kučera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press.
Google Scholar
Levy, Y. (1983). It’s frogs all the way down. Cognition,
15, 73–93.
Article
Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 42, Serial No. 174.
Maratsos, M. (1983). Some current issues in the study of the acquisition of grammar. In P. Mussen, J. H. Flavell, & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 709–777). New York: Wiley.
Monaghan, P., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). The differential contribution of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorization. Cognition,
96, 143–182.
Article
Google Scholar
Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2007). The Phonological-Distributional Coherence Hypothesis: Cross-linguistic evidence in language acquisition. Cognitive Psychology, 55(4), 259–305.
Article
Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (1996). Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L., Meier, R. P., & Newport, E. L. (1987). Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language. Cognitive Psychology,
19, 498–550.
Article
Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L., Shi, R., & Allopenna, P. (1996). Perceptual bases of rudimentary categories: Towards a broader conceptualization of bootstrapping. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 263–283). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of nonadjacent dependencies. Cognitive Psychology,
48, 127–162.
Article
Google Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology,
33, 637–649.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Dowd, S. C. (1984). Does vocabulary decline qualitatively in old age? Educational Gerontology,
10, 357–368.
Article
Google Scholar
Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing ability. Language and Speech,
31, 337–355.
Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996a). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science,
274, 1926–1928.
Article
Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1996b). Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language,
35, 606–621.
Article
Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science,
8, 101–195.
Article
Google Scholar
Schiff, R., & Raveh, M. (2007). Deficient morphological processing in adults with developmental dyslexia: Another barrier to efficient word recognition? Dyslexia,
13, 110–129.
Article
Google Scholar
Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2007). Conflicting cues in marking plural adjectives in Hebrew. Paper Presented at XIV Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Prague.
Shi, R., Morgan, J., & Allopenna, P. (1998). Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: A cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Child Language,
25, 169–201.
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, P. T., & Baker, R. G. (1976). The influence of English spelling patterns on pronunciation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour,
15, 267–285.
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, P. T., Baker, R. G., & Groat, A. (1982). Spelling as a source of information about children’s linguistic knowledge. British Journal of Psychology,
73, 339–350.
Google Scholar
Soreno, J. A., & Jongman, A. (1990). Phonological and form class relations in the lexicon. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,
19, 387–404.
Article
Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1988). Units in reading and spelling. Journal of Memory and Language,
27, 466–477.
Article
Google Scholar
Tucker, G. R., Lambert, W. E., Rigault, A., & Segalowitz, N. (1968). A psychological investigation of French speakers’ skill with grammatical gender. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 7, 312–316.
Article
Google Scholar
Valtin, R. (1984). The development of metalinguistic awareness in children learning to read and write. In J. Downing & R. Valtin (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 207–226). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Google Scholar
Wilson, M. D. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine readable dictionary, version 2. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers,
20, 6–11.
Google Scholar