Abstract
The critical audit of Q methodology by Kampen and Tamás contains many errors of fact and understanding—indeed, a resistance to understanding that is compared to the Medicis’ stance toward Galileo. Following a brief historical summary of similar ill-advised critiques of Q methodology in the 80 years since its introduction, responses are presented to various of the points raised: on the nature of subjectivity, the universe of subjective communicability (concourse) and samples drawn from it, the role of factor analysis and factor interpretation, the forced Q-sort distribution, the ratio between the number of participants and the number of statements in the Q sample, and sources of researcher bias.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Notes
We would be remiss were we not to mention two other Q journals (Journal of Human Subjectivity and Q-Methodology and Theory, the latter in Korean) as well as several other books on Q technique and its methodology—-by Block (2008), Iliescu (2005), Khoshgooyanfard (2008), Kim (2008), Kim (2007), McKeown and Thomas (2013), Prasith-rathsint and Sookasame (2007), Said and Stricklin (2013), Thorsen and Allgood (2010) and Watts and Stenner (2012)—-of which Kampen and Tamás are apparently unaware, as they apparently also are of the 60 chapters and dozen encyclopedia entries on Q methodology (e.g., Brown and Good 2010, Smith 2001, pp. 319–343 ), not to mention the more than 50 books that utilize Q (e.g., Kanra 2009).
A search in SCOPUS using the search string TITLE-ABS-KEY(Q-method OR Q-sort OR Q-methodology OR Q-methodological OR “Q method” OR “Q sort” OR “Q methodology” OR “Q methodological”) identified 1,922 articles. The number of published articles increased from an average of 10 per year in the years up to 1990, to 35 in the years 1991–2000, and 92 in the years 2001–2013. These articles were published in source titles from the life sciences, health sciences, physical sciences, and social sciences and humanities, with psychology (21.8 %), medicine (19.4 %), and social sciences (15.3 %) as the principal subject areas.
In September 2013, the 29th annual Q conference was held in Kampen and Tamás’s country of residence, the Netherlands (see http://qmethod.org/), offering them an easy opportunity to share and discuss their concerns with the international Q community (among attendants, scholars from 12 universities and research organizations in the Netherlands).
Unknown to one another, Danielson and Brown were reviewers of two separate earlier versions and rendered critical commentary that Kampen and Tamás have apparently elected not to take into account.
By our count, Kampen and Tamás’s search procedure missed at least 30 Q publications appearing in 2010 (10 of them in an edited book on Q methodology) and that were announced on the Q-Method electronic discussion list, which has a subscribership of more than 800 scholars (but not Kampen or Tamás).
We have never before encountered this argument and have no idea where Kampen and Tamás got it, but it is important to note that the data of Q methodology are not responses to individual statements alone, but more importantly in their relationships, as when they are rank-ordered (as in Q sorting). In this connection, Brown (1980, pp. 265–267) has shown that for \(N\) \(=\) 33 statements (which is below average in size), there were more than 44 trillion different Q sorts possible, or more than 6,000 times as many different Q sorts as there are humans on Earth. Not all of these different ways are uncorrelated, of course, but the numbers do seem to leave enough maneuvering room for the usual study, which typically employs fewer than 50 participants.
Pett et al. (2003, pp. 47–48) state that there is no empirical evidence on the ratio of number of subjects to number of items that is required for undertaking factor analysis and that little agreement exists among authorities in factor analysis about rules of thumb to be used.
Kampen and Tamás elect not to examine Q methodology’s parallels to quantum theory (Sect. 1), which is a pity since their implicit commitment to a Newtonian conception of science (as was the case with Wittenborn before them) goes some distance in explaining their inability to achieve a substantial grasp of Q methodology. A glimpse into Q’s quantum connection can be gotten by examining the series on “William James, Niels Bohr, and Complementarity,” beginning with Stephenson ’s (1986) first of five articles in Psychological Record.
References
Baker, R., van Exel, J., Mason, H., Stricklin, M.: Connecting Q and surveys: a test of three methods to explore factor membership in a large sample. Operant. Subject. 34, 38–58 (2010)
Baker, R., Wildman, J., Mason, H., Donaldson, C.: Q-ing for health–a new approach to eliciting the public’s views on health care resource allocation. Health Econ. 23, 283–297 (2014)
Billard, S.: How Q methodology can be democratized. Fem. Psychol. 9, 357–366 (1999)
Block, J.: The Q-Sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psychiatric Research. Thomas, Springfield (1961)
Block, J.: The Q-Sort in Character Appraisal: Encoding Subjective Impressions of Persons Quantitatively. American Psychological Association, Washington (2008)
Bolland, J.M.: The search for structure: an alternative to the forced Q-sort technique. Polit. Methodol. 11(1–2), 91–107 (1985)
Brown, H.I.: Galileo on the telescope and the eye. J. Hist. Ideas 46, 487–501 (1985)
Brown, S.R.: Political Subjectivity: Applications of Q Methodology in Political Science. Yale University Press, New Haven (1980)
Brown, S.R.: Q methodology and naturalistic subjectivity. In: Midgley, B.D., Morris, E.K. (eds.) Modern Perspectives on J.R. Kantor and Interbehaviorism, pp. 251–268. Context Press, Reno (2006)
Brown, S.R., Good, J.M.M.: Q methodology. In: Salkind, N.J. (ed.) Encyclopedia of Research Design, vol. 3, pp. 1149–1155. Sage, Thousand Oaks (2010)
Burt, C.L., Stephenson, W.: Alternative views on correlations between persons. Psychometrika 4, 269–281 (1939)
Cartwright, D.S.: A computational procedure for tau correlation. Psychometrika 22, 97–104 (1957)
Cattell, R.B.: On the disuse and misuse of P, Q, Qs and O techniques in clinical psychology. J. Clin. Psychol. 7, 203–214 (1951)
Conover, P.J., Feldman, S.: Group identification, values, and the nature of political beliefs. Am. Polit. Q. 12, 151–175 (1984)
Cragan, J.F., Shields, D.C.: The identifying characteristics of public fire safety educators: an empirical analysis. In: Cragan, J.F., Shields, D.C. (eds.) Applied Communication Research: A Dramatistic Approach, pp. 219–234. Waveland, Prospect Heights (1981)
Cronbach, L.J., Gleser, G.C.: Review of the study of behavior: Q-technique and its methodology, by W. Stephenson. Psychometrika 19, 327–330 (1954)
Cuppen, E., Breukers, S., Hisschemöller, M., Bergsma, E.: Q methodology to select participants for a stakeholder dialogue on energy options from biomass in the Netherlands. Ecol. Econ. 69, 579–591 (2010)
Danielson, S.: Q method and surveys: three ways to combine Q and R. Field Methods 21, 219–237 (2009)
De Graaf, G., van Exel, J.: Using Q-methodology in administrative ethics. Public Integr. 11, 63–78 (2009)
Garrard, J., Hausman, W.: The priority sort: an empirical approach to program planning and evaluation. Am. J. Soc. Psychiatry, 5(5), 29–36 (1985). [Reprinted. In: D.S. Cordray M.W. Lipsey (eds.): Evaluation Studies Review Annual, vol. 11, pp. 279–286. Sage, Beverly Hills (1986)].
Gould, P.: A new Q too? Operant Subject. 8, 42–53 (1985)
Howard, L.W.: Quest-sort: a paper-and-pencil alternative to card-sorting Q samples. Operant Subject. 19, 12–22 (1995–1996)
Iliescu, D.: Metodologia Q. Communicare.ro, Budapest (2005). [Romanian]
Jackson, D.M., Bidwell, C.E.: A modification of Q-technique. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 19, 221–232 (1959)
Jedeloo, S., van Staa, A., Latour, J.M., van Exel, N.J.A.: Preferences for health care and self-management among Dutch adolescents with chronic conditions: A Q-methodological investigation. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 47, 593–603 (2010)
Johnson, R.M.: Q analysis of large samples. J. Mark. Res. 7, 104–105 (1970)
Kampen, J., Tamás, P.: Overly ambitious: contributions and current status of Q methodology. Qual. Quan. (2014). doi:10.1007/s11135-013-9944-z
Kanra, B.: Islam, Democracy and Dialogue in Turkey: Deliberating in Divided Societies. Ashgate, Farnham (2009)
Khoshgooyanfard, A.R.: Q-methodology. Research Center of IRIB (Islamic Republic of Iran Broadcasting) Publication, Tehran (2008). [Persian]
Killam, L.A., Montgomery, P., Luhanga, F.L., Adamic, P., Carter, L.M.: Views on unsafe nursing students in clinical learning. Int. J. Nurs. Educ. Scholarsh. 7(1), article 36 (2010)
Kim, H.K.: Q Methodology: Philosophy of Science, Theories, Analysis, and Application. CommunicationBooks, Seoul (2008). [Korean]
Kim, S.E.: Q Methodology and Social Sciences. Goldwell, Busan (2007). [Korean]
Kitzinger, C.: The Social Construction of Lesbianism (Inquiries in Social Construction series). Sage, London (1987)
Loevinger, J.: Person and population as psychometric concepts. Psychol. Rev. 72, 143–155 (1965)
McKeown, B.F., Thomas, D.B.: Q methodology (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences Series), vol. 66, 2nd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks (2013)
Mowrer, O.H.: “Q-technique”–description, history, and critique. In: Mowrer, O.H. (ed.) Psychotherapy, pp. 316–375. Ronald, New York (1953)
Nahinsky, I.E.: A Q sort analysis of variance involving the dimensions of sorts, groups, and items. J. Exp. Educ. 35(3), 36–41 (1967)
Neff, W.S., Cohen, J.: A method for the analysis of the structure and internal consistency of Q-sort arrays. Psychol. Bull. 68, 361–368 (1967)
Peterson, R.S., Owens, P.D., Martorana, P.V.: The group dynamics Q-sort in organizational research: a new method for studying familiar problems. Organ. Res. Methods 2, 107–139 (1999)
Pett, M.A., Lackey, N.R., Sullivan, J.J.: Making Sense of Factor Analysis: The Use of Factor Analysis for Instrument Development in Health Care Research. Sage, Thousand Oaks (2003)
Phillips, E.L., Raiford, A., El-Batrawi, S.: The Q sort reevaluated. J. Consult. Psychol. 29, 422–425 (1965)
Prasith-rathsint, S., Sookasame, K.: Variety of Innovative Research Methods: Q Methodology: A Scientific Study of Subjectivity (Concepts, Theory and Application). Samlada, Bangkok (2007). [Thai]
Sabini, J.B., Silver, M.: Some senses of subjective. In: Secord, P.F. (ed.) Explaining Human Behavior: Consciousness, Human Action and Social Structure, pp. 71–91. Sage, Beverly Hills (1982)
Said, G., Stricklin, M. (eds.): In: Anais da 1\(^{o}\) conferencia internacional sobre metodologia Q: Analises qualitative e quantitative na pesquisa cientfica. EDUFPI, Teresina, Piauí, Brazil (2013)
Smith, N.W.: Current Systems in Psychology: History, Theory, Research, and Applications. Wadsworth/Thomson Learning, Belmont (2001)
Stainton Rogers, W.: Explaining Health and Illness: An Exploration of Diversity. Harvester Wheatsheaf, Exeter (1991)
Stenner, P. (Ed.) (2008–2009). Special Issue: Q and constructivism: Between discursive practice and subjective process. Operant Subjectivity, 32, entire issue.
Stephenson, W.: Technique of factor analysis. Nature 136, 297 (1935a)
Stephenson, W.: Correlating persons instead of tests. Character Pers. 4, 17–24 (1935b)
Stephenson, W.: The foundations of psychometry: four factor systems. Psychometrika 1, 195–209 (1936)
Stephenson, W.: A note on Professor R.B. Cattell’s methodological adumbrations. J. Clin. Psychol. 8, 206–207 (1952)
Stephenson, W.: The Study of Behavior: Q-Technique and Its Methodology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1953)
Stephenson, W.: Comments on Cronbach and Gleser’s review of: The study of behavior: Q-technique and its methodology. Psychometrika 19, 331–333 (1954)
Stephenson, W.: The Play Theory of Mass Communication. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1967)
Stephenson, W.: Concourse theory of communication. Communication 3, 21–40 (1978)
Stephenson, W.: Against interpretation. Operant Subject. 6, 73–125 (1983)
Stephenson, W.: William James, Niels Bohr, and complementarity: I-Concepts. Psychol. Record 36, 519–527 (1986)
Sundland, D.M.: The construction of Q-sorts: a criticism. Psychol. Rev. 69, 62–64 (1962)
Thomas, D.B., Baas, L.R.: The issue of generalization in Q methodology: “Reliable schematics” revisited. Operant Subject. 16, 18–36 (1992–1993)
Thompson, B., Frankiewicz, R.G., Ward, G.R.: Cross-technique validation of attitude measures. Operant Subject. 6, 37–50 (1983)
Thorsen, A.A., Allgood, E. (eds.): [Q-Metodologi: En Velegnet Måte å utforske Subjektivitet.] Trondheim. Tapir Academic Press, Norway (2010)
Van Exel, N.J.A., de Graaf, G., Brouwer, W.B.F.: Give me a break! Associations between informal caregivers’ attitudes toward respite care and characteristics of caregivers, care recipients and the care giving situation. Health Policy 88, 73–87 (2008)
Vermaire, J.H., Hoogstraten, J., Van Loveren, C., Poorterman, J.H.G., Van Exel, N.J.A.: Attitudes towards oral health among parents of 6-year-old children at risk of developing caries. Community Dent. Oral Epidemiol. 38, 507–520 (2010)
Vogel, J., Lowham, E.: Building consensus for constructive action: a study of perspectives on natural resource management. J. For. 105, 20–26 (2007)
Watts, S., Stenner, P.: Doing Q methodology: theory, method, and interpretation. Qual. Res. Psychol. 2, 67–91 (2005)
Watts, S., Stenner, P.: Doing Q Methodological Research: Theory, Method and Interpretation. Sage, Thousand Oaks (2012)
Wittenborn, J.R.: Contributions and current status of Q methodology. Psychol. Bull. 58, 132–142 (1961)
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Brown, S.R., Danielson, S. & van Exel, J. Overly ambitious critics and the Medici Effect: a reply to Kampen and Tamás. Qual Quant 49, 523–537 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-014-0007-x
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-014-0007-x
Keywords
- Q methodology
- R methodology
- Subjectivity
- Factor analysis