Quantitative Marketing and Economics

, Volume 10, Issue 1, pp 27–62 | Cite as

An empirical analysis of individual level casino gambling behavior

Article

Abstract

Gambling and gaming is a very large industry in the United States with about one-third of all adults participating in it on a regular basis. Using novel and unique behavioral data from a panel of casino gamblers, this paper investigates three aspects of consumer behavior in this domain. The first is that consumers are addicted to gambling, the second that they act on “irrational” beliefs, and the third that they are influenced by marketing activity that attempts to influence their gambling behavior. We use the interrelated consumer decisions to play (gamble) and the amount bet in a casino setting to focus on addiction using the standard economic definition of addiction. We test for two irrational behaviors, the “gambler’s fallacy” and the “hot hand myth”—our research represents the first test for these behaviors using disaggregate data in a real (as opposed to a laboratory) setting. Finally, we look at the effect of marketing instruments on the both the decision to play and the amount bet. Using hierarchical Bayesian methods to pin down individual-level parameters, we find that about 8% of the consumers in our sample can be classified as addicted. We find support in our data for the gambler’s fallacy, but not for the hot hand myth. We find that marketing instruments positively affect gambling behavior, and that consumers who are more addicted are also affected by marketing to a greater extent. Specifically, the long-run marketing response is about twice as high for the more addicted consumers.

Keywords

Gambling behavior Addiction Selection models Casino gaming and gambling Slots Hierarchical Bayes methods 

JEL Classification

M31 M37 D03 C33 C11 

References

  1. Ayton, P., & Fischer, I. (2004). The hot hand fallacy and the gambler’s fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? Memory and Cognition, 32(8), 1369–1378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Becker, G. S., Grossman, M., & Murphy, K. M. (1994). An empirical analysis of cigarette addiction. American Economic Review, 84(3), 396–418.Google Scholar
  3. Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy, 96(4), 675–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chaloupka, F. (1991). Rational addictive behavior and cigarette smoking. Journal of Political Economy, 99(4), 722–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chib, S., & Greenberg, E. (1995). Understanding the metropolis Hastings algorithm. American Statistician, 49, 327–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clotfelter, C. T., & Cook, P. J. (1993). The gambler’s fallacy in lottery play. Management Science, 39(12), 1521–1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Croson, R., & Sundali, J. (2005). The gambler’s fallacy and the hot hand: Empirical data from casinos. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30(3), 195–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gelfand, A. E., & Smith, A. F. M. (1990). Sampling-based approaches to calculating marginal densities. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 85(410), 398–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gruber, J., & Koszegi, B. (2001). Is addiction rational? Theory and evidence. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(4), 1261–1303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Guryan, J., & Kearney, M. S. (2005). Lucky stores, gambling and addiction: Empirical evidence from state lottery sales. NBER Working Paper 11287.Google Scholar
  12. Guryan, J., & Kearney, M. S. (2008). Gambling at lucky stores: Empirical evidence from state lottery sales. American Economic Review, 98(1), 458–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 79–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica, 47(1), 153–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Heckman, J. J. (1981). Statistical models for discrete panel data. In C. F. Manski & D. L. McFadden (Ed.), Structural analysis of discrete data and econometric applications. MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Jones, J. (2004). Profile of the American gambler. Available at http://www.harrahs.com/about_us/survey/2004_Survey.pdf. Last accessed on 15 June 2006.
  17. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kearney, M. S. (2005). The economic winners and losers of legalized gambling. National Tax Journal, 58, 281–302.Google Scholar
  19. Metzger, M. (1984). Biases in betting: An application of laboratory findings. Psychological Reports, 56, 883–888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Mullainathan, S. (2002). Thinking through categories. Working Paper, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  21. Olekalns, N., & Bardsley, P. (1996). Rational addiction to caffeine: An analysis of coffee consumption. Journal of Political Economy, 104(5), 1100–1104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Peele, S. (1985). The meaning of addiction: Compulsive experience and its interpretation. Lexington, Mass.Google Scholar
  23. Pollak, R. A. (1970). Habit formation and dynamic demand functions. Journal of Political Economy, 78(4), 745–763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Potenza, M. N., Kosten, T. R., & Rounsaville, B. J. (2001). Pathological gambling. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286(2), 141–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Roy, R., Chintagunta, P. K., & Haldar, S. (1996). A Framework for investigating habits: “The Hand of the Past,” and heterogeneity in dynamic demand choice. Marketing Science, 15(3), 280–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Stigler, G. J., & Becker, G. S. (1977). De Gustibus Non Est Disputandum. American Economic Review, 67(2), 76–90.Google Scholar
  27. Tanner, T., & Wong, W. (1987). The calculation of posterior distributions by data augmentation. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82, 528–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Terrell, D. (1994). A test of the gambler’s fallacy: Evidence from parimutuel games. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 8, 309–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 105–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Graduate School of BusinessStanford UniversityStanfordUSA
  2. 2.Ross School of BusinessUniversity of MichiganAnn ArborUSA

Personalised recommendations