Skip to main content
Log in

Fast, frugal and correct? An experimental study on the influence of time scarcity and quantity of information on the voter decision making process

  • Published:
Public Choice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Classical theories of political representation claim that high levels of political knowledge and awareness across the electorate are crucial for a well-functioning democracy. In real life, though, the actual amount of political information possessed by voters is small and unevenly distributed. In addition, electoral decision making is complex and often characterized by severe time pressure. This study therefore experimentally tests the theoretical framework of “fast and frugal” voters and the “less-is-more effect” (Gigerenzer and Goldstein in Psychol Rev 103(4):650–669, 1996), which expects that in some instances low levels of political knowledge may actually result in equally good or even better outcomes than in the case of well-informed decisions. It is hence assumed that employing specific cognitive shortcuts can help voters make good and quick decisions, even with a lack of information. We have used a laboratory experiment to create an environment characterized by severe time pressure that allowed for only shallow information search and limited comparisons across alternatives. Subjects were able to base their decisions only on a small number of criteria operationalized by candidates’ positions on current political issues with various levels of perceived salience. Voters appear to simplify decision making by focusing mainly on those policy attitudes they consider important. Consequently, it seems that shifting the decision-making strategy from accuracy to efficiency has no effect on the final outcome.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. According to a survey made by the agency STEM/MARK, one-quarter of voters participating in elections to the Czech Republic’s Chamber of Deputies in 2013 decided at the last minute. Seventy-eight percent of them, who decided to participate in the election only a week beforehand, chose their candidates during that week or in the voting booth (STEM/MARK 2013).

  2. Kuklinski and Quirk (2000b), nevertheless, argue that such a decision-making mechanism can sufficiently operate in various domains of life, but that it is unsuited for politics.

  3. “Correct vote” is not the only way of evaluating whether a decision fits that definition or not. For example, Bartels (1996) and Delli Carpini and Keeter (1996) also focused on the differences between decisions made by more and less informed voters. They, however, did not use the term “correct decision”.

  4. Baum and Jamison (2016) employed the term “consistent” voting because they strive to avoid the normative connotations ascribed to the term “correct vote”.

  5. Our assumptions are based on a classical spatial model claiming that the relationships between political actors can be represented by proximity and distance. The basis for political success in this model is the smallest possible distance of the candidate from the voters (Downs 1957). However, it is important to note that it’s not the only way to model spatial political preferences. Another approach is represented by so-called directional models, which work not only with positions of voters and candidates, but they include also the status quo referential point. Such models suggest that voters’ utility of choice is not given by the absolute distance between voters’ and candidates’ positions, but by their directions, determined by the referential point (whether both voter and candidate are on the same side of the simple, dichotomous, policy space) (Claasen 2007). The most prominent type of directional models was introduced by Rabinowitz and Macdonald (1989). They propose that the utility associated with a choice depends on the intensity of a voter’s and a candidate’s relationship to the referential point (the further they both are in the same direction to the referential point, the larger is the voter’s utility).

  6. All texts in the experiment were originally in Czech. For the purposes of this article they were translated into English.

  7. See “Appendix” for the sample of possible candidate positions on the issue of involvement of the Czech Republic in the fight against ISIS. Full texts of other issues are available as electronic supplementary material (Online Appendix).

  8. The experiment did not follow what would happen if even greater distances between the voter and the candidate were possible. We assume that such a treatment probably would raise the likelihood of casting a correct vote, since the greater attitudinal distance between the candidates would make it easier for the voter to do so. When designing the experiment, however, we had to take into consideration several aspects. First, we had to make sure that the selection of the correct candidate was not the result of chance, so we worked with up to three candidates. At the same time, we were not able to extend the range of the scale to more than five possible positions because the differences between the verbal expressions of the positions would be too small, which could cause an unclear decision-making situation and also jeopardize the reliability of the measurement. Since we wanted to ensure the same pattern of candidate placement on each issue and use a five-degree scale, we could not work with more extreme positions.

  9. Differences in average salience of each issue between experimental groups were not statistically significant (the only exception was the “pensions” issue).

  10. We also checked the assumption that on the most salient issues (the top two), voters take the most extreme positions (1 or 5). We found that for the time-constrained group the assumption holds true in 80% of the cases, and in 74.3% of the cases for the group without a time constraint. The positions of the candidates were then modeled according to the predesigned pattern described in Sect. 7 above.

  11. We also checked for multicollinearity. Tests shows that multicollinearity is not present in our data (tolerance = 0.258, VIF = 3.874). In addition, we checked group characteristics in different dimensions (sex, age, sophistication) to ensure that random assignment of participants to each experimental condition worked. The results indicate that the two groups do not differ significantly along those dimensions.

References

  • Baldassarri, D. (2013). The simple art of voting: The cognitive shortcuts of Italian voters. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. (1996). Uninformed votes: Information effects in presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science, 40(14), 194–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, M., & Jamison, A. (2016). The Oprah effect: How soft news helps inattentive citizens vote consistently. The Journal of Politics, 68(4), 946–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berelson, B., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chytilek, R., & Eibl, O. (2011). České politické strany v politickém prostoru. Sociologický časopis, 47(1), 61–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claasen, R. L. (2007). Ideology and evaluation in an experimental setting: Comparing the proximity and the directional models. Political Research Quarterly, 60(2), 263–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Czerlinski, J., Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. (1999). How good are simple heuristics? In G. Gigerenzer & P. Todd (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, R. A. (1971). Polyarchy: Participation and opposition. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. Boston: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2005). Does political information matter? Political Communication, 22(4), 515–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher, U. (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (2013). Social cognition: From brains to culture (1st ed.). Los Angeles: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fournier, P., Blais, A., Nadeau, R., Gidengil, E., & Nevitte, N. (2003). Issue importance and performance voting. Political Behavior, 25(1), 51–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2001). The adaptive toolbox. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103(4), 650–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graefe, A., & Armstrong, S. (2010). Predicting elections from the most important issue: A test of the take-the-best heuristic. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(1), 41–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graefe, A., & Armstrong, S. (2014). Forecasts of the 2012 US presidential election based on candidates’ perceived competence in handling the most important issue. Political Science Research and Methods, 2(1), 141–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Krosnick, J. A., Visser, P. S., & Boninger, D. S. (2005). Attitude importance and the accumulation of attitude-relevant knowledge in memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 749–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Walker, J. (2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. The Journal of Politics, 70(1), 186–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Junn, J. (1991). Participation and political knowledge. In W. Crotty (Ed.), Political participation and American democracy. New York: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, K. (1985). Ideology in the 1980 election: Ideological sophistication does matter. The Journal of Politics, 47(3), 828–853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. (1990). Government policy and citizen passion: A study of issue publics in contemporary America. Political Behavior, 12(1), 59–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., & Milburn, M. A. (1990). Psychological determinants of political opinionation. Social Cognition, 8(1), 49–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J., & Quirk, P. (2000a). A cognitive-sciences perspective: Hard wiring, evolution, and the tasks of citizenship. In A. Lupia, M. Mccubbins, & S. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J., & Quirk, P. (2000b). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics and mass opinion. In A. Lupia, M. Mccubbins, & S. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lanoue, D. (1992). One that made a difference: Cognitive consistency, political knowledge, and the 1980 presidential debate. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 56(2), 168–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R., & Redlawsk, D. (1997). Voting correctly. The American Political Science Review, 91(3), 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R., & Redlawsk, D. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing during election campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Laver, M. (2001). Position and salience in the policies of political actors. In M. Laver (Ed.), Estimating the policy positions of political actors. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., & Mccubbins, M. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know? (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martignon, L. (2001). Comparing fast and frugal heuristics and optimal models. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, R. M. (2013). Measuring “correct voting” using comparative manifestos project data. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion, 23(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murr, A. (2012). A tournament of voters’ decision making rules: Comparing four models of citizen forecasting. Paper prepared for presentation at the Elections, Public Opinion and Parties conference, University of Oxford, 7th–9th September 2012 [online]. http://www.sociology.ox.ac.uk/materials/documents/epop/papers/MurrATournamentOfVotersDecisionMakingRulesEPOP2012.pdf.

  • Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1992). The rational public: fifty years of trends in Americans’ policy preferences (p. 1992). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J., & Bettman, J. (2001). Preferential choice and adaptive strategy use. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J., Bettman, J., & Johnson, E. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Popkin, S. (1994). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitz, G., & Macdonald, S. (1989). A directional theory of issue voting. American Political Science Review, 83(1), 93–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. (2004). What voters do: Information search during election campaigns. Political Psychology, 25(4), 595–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (1999). When do people use simple heuristics, and how can we tell? In G. Gigerenzer & P. Todd (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1957). Rational choice and the structure of environments. Psychological Review, 63(2), 129–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STEM/MARK. (2013). Informace z výskumu STEM Trendy 11/2013. https://www.stem.cz/wp-content/uploads/2015/12/2865_1723.pdf.

  • Todd, P. (2001). Fast and frugal heuristics for environmentally bounded minds. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomz, M., & Van Houweling, R. P. (2008). Candidate positioning and voter choice. American Political Science Review, 102(03), 303–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank William F. Shughart II, the Editor in Chief, and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable and thoughtful suggestions, which helped us to improve the quality of the paper. Also many thanks to Peter Voda, Petra Svačinová, Peter Spáč and Lenka Hrbková for their comments and fruitful discussion upon manuscript. Early version of this paper was presented at the 2016 Annual Scientific Meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology (ISPP) in Warsaw, Poland. This publication was written at Masaryk University with the support of the Specific University Research Grand provided by the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports of the Czech Republic (MUNI/A/0850/2017).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michal Tóth.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 25 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

A sample of possible candidate positions on the issue of involvement of the Czech Republic in the fight against ISIS (the full set of possible positions on all of the issues is available as electronic supplementary material in the Online Appendix).

  1. 1.

    The Islamic State is the biggest security threat for today’s Europe. The Czech Republic must therefore strive for the unconditional launch of a direct military ground operation. The deployment of the army is the most effective way to suppress the expansionism of the Islamic State, and the Czech Republic should therefore actively be involved in this effort by sending ground troops.

  2. 2.

    The Czech Republic should proceed vigorously in the fight against the Islamic State, but with regard to the security of its citizens. Military intervention is therefore the right step; however, it should be implemented in the form of air raids that do not pose such a threat to soldiers as a ground operation. The Czech Republic should therefore be involved in the fight against ISIS, in particular by deploying its fighter jets.

  3. 3.

    The aim of the international effort should be, in particular, to support the Arab States, which can lead the fight against the Islamic State most effectively. The Czech Republic should participate in this endeavor but only by delivering ammunition and weapons to Kurdish fighters and the Iraqi army, providing humanitarian aid, or engaging in stabilization activities in Iraq.

  4. 4.

    Engaging in a direct struggle against the Islamic State is ineffective given the abbilities of the Czech Republic. The United States and its allies have significantly better capacity to intervene effectively against the ISIS. Similar activities would be too demanding and costly for the Czech Republic. Its role should therefore be primarily to participate in the strengthening of the EU’s internal protection.

  5. 5.

    The Czech Republic should not participate in the fight against the Islamic State at all. By such action, the Czech Republic’s internal security and the lives of its citizens would be jeopardized. Moreover, the Czech Republic has no share in the destabilization of the Middle East region, and in no way should it participate in military or non-military activities against the ISIS.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tóth, M., Chytilek, R. Fast, frugal and correct? An experimental study on the influence of time scarcity and quantity of information on the voter decision making process. Public Choice 177, 67–86 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11127-018-0587-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11127-018-0587-4

Keywords

Navigation