Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of Abstinence from Social Media on Time Perception: Differences between Low- and At-Risk for Social Media “Addiction” Groups

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Psychiatric Quarterly Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Time distortion is a hallmark feature of addictive behaviors including excessive technology use. It has clinically significant implications for diagnosis and treatment. Additional information on such distortions after prolonged abstinence from technology use is needed. We seek to examine differences in the effects of several days of abstinence on time-distortion in two groups: social media users who are at-risk and those who are at low risk for social media “addiction.” To examine this, we employed a randomized, two group, pre (t1) - post (t2) design. Both groups completed survey tasks that cued social media use at t1 and at t2. Between t1 and t2, the treatment group (n = 294) abstained from social media use for up to one week (less if they “broke” and decided to resume use), and the control group (n = 121) did not. Results indicated that low-risk individuals in both the treatment and control groups presented downward time bias at t1; at-risk individuals presented non-significant upward bias. After abstinence, both low- and at- risk individuals in the treatment group presented upward time distortion. This effect did not take place in the control group; low-risk users still presented significant downward bias at t2. The post-abstinence increase in time distortion was significantly more pronounced in at-risk users. These differences between pre- and post-abstinence time distortion patterns in normal and at-risk-for-“addiction” social media users can be used for adjusting and interpreting self-reports related to addictive uses of technologies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the original cutoff study [13], Latent Profile Analysis detected this cutoff as the lower bound of a cluster of social media users with significantly elevated levels of symptoms, social media use, and depressive symptoms compared to the rest of the population, as well as reduced self-esteem compared to the rest of the population. The cutoff produced 83% sensitivity, 99% specificity, 73% positive predictive value, 99% Negative Predictive Value, and 98% accuracy.

References

  1. Wittmann M, Leland DS, Churan J, Paulus MP. Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90(2–3):183–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Lin YH, Lin YC, Lee YH, Lin PH, Lin SH, Chang LR, et al. Time distortion associated with smartphone addiction: identifying smartphone addiction via a mobile application (app). J Psychiatr Res. 2015;65:139–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.04.003.

  3. Rau PLP, Peng SY, Yang CC. Time distortion for expert and novice online game players. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2006;9(4):396–403. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Turel O, Brevers D, Bechara A. Time distortion when users at-risk for social media addiction engage in non-social media tasks. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;97:84–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.11.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Turel O. Quitting the use of a habituated hedonic information system: a theoretical model and empirical examination of Facebook users. European Journal of Information Systems, 2015;24(4):431–446. https://doi.org/10.1057/ejis.2014.19.

  6. Turel O. Untangling the complex role of guilt in rational decisions to discontinue the use of a hedonic Information System. European Journal of Information Systems, 2016;25(5):432–447. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41303-016-0002-5.

  7. Turel O, He, Xue, Xiao, Bechara. Examination of neural systems sub-serving Facebook "addiction". Psychological Reports, 2014;115(3):675–695. https://doi.org/10.2466/18.PR0.115c31z8.

  8. Turel O, Mouttapa, Donato. Preventing problematic Internet use through video-based interventions: a theoretical model and empirical test. Behaviour & Information Technology, 2015;34(4):349–362. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2014.936041.

  9. Turel O, Poppa, Gil-Or. Neuroticism Magnifies the Detrimental Association between Social Media Addiction Symptoms and Wellbeing in Women, but Not in Men: a three-Way Moderation Model. Psychiatric Quarterly, 2018;89(3):605–619. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-018-9563-x.

  10. Turel O, Qahri-Saremi. Problematic use of social networking sites: Antecedents and consequence from a dual system theory perspective. Journal of Management Information Systems, 2016;33(4):1087–1116.

  11. Turel O, Serenko, Giles. Integrating technology addiction and use: An empirical investigation of online auction sites. MIS Quarterly, 2011;35(4):1043–1061.

  12. Andreassen CS, Pallesen S, Griffiths MD. The relationship between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem: findings from a large national survey. Addict Behav. 2017;64:287–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Banyai F, Zsila A, Kiraly O, Maraz A, Elekes Z, Griffiths MD, et al. Problematic social media use: results from a large-scale nationally representative adolescent sample. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169839. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169839.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. He, Turel O, Bechara. Association of excessive social media use with abnormal white matter integrity of the corpus callosum. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 2018;278;42–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.06.008.

  15. Turel O, He, Brevers, Bechara. Social networking sites use and the morphology of a social-semantic brain network. Social Neuroscience, 2018;13(5):628–636. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1382387.

  16. Klein LC, Corwin EJ, Stine MM. Smoking abstinence impairs time estimation accuracy in cigarette smokers. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2003;37(1):90–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Merson F, Perriot J. Social deprivation and time perception, the impact on smoking cessation. Sante Publique. 2011;23(5):359–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Sayette MA, Loewenstein G, Kirchner TR, Travis T. Effects of smoking urge on temporal cognition. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(1):88–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164x.19.1.88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Fraisse P. Perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev Psychol. 1984;35(1):1–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wei L, Zhang S, Turel O, Bechara A, He Q. Tripartite neurocognitive model of internet gaming disorder. Front Psychol. 2017;8(285):1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Wittmann M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(1):7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Turel O, Bechara A. A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Front Psychol. 2016;7(Article 601):1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Development of a Facebook addiction scale. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(2):501–17. https://doi.org/10.2466/02.09.18.PR0.110.2.501-517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Wittmann M. In: Butler E, editor. Felt time: the psychology of how we perceive time. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wölfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: an interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:252–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Platt JJ. Heroin Addiction: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2nd ed. Malabar: Robert E. Kreiger; 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Lortie CL, Guitton MJ. Internet addiction assessment tools: dimensional structure and methodological status. Addiction. 2013;108(7):1207–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

OT and DC designed the study, collected data, ran analyses and wrote a draft. It was edited and approved by OT and DC.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ofir Turel.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Ofir Turel declares that he has no conflict of interest. Daniel Cavagnaro declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Turel, O., R. Cavagnaro, D. Effect of Abstinence from Social Media on Time Perception: Differences between Low- and At-Risk for Social Media “Addiction” Groups. Psychiatr Q 90, 217–227 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-018-9614-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-018-9614-3

Keywords

Navigation