Skip to main content
Log in

CueAnon: What QAnon Signals About Congressional Candidates and What it Costs Them

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most research investigates why the public embraces conspiracy theories, but few studies empirically examine how Americans evaluate the politicians who do. We argued that politicians portrayed as supporting QAnon would garner negative mainstream media attention, but this coverage could increase their name recognition and signal positive attributes to voters with low trust in media who would feel warmer toward those candidates. Although we confirm that candidates friendly toward QAnon receive more negative media coverage, our nationally-representative vignette experiment reveals that QAnon support decreases favorability toward candidates, even among seemingly sympathetic sub-populations. A follow-up conjoint experiment, varying whether candidates support QAnon, replicates these findings. This paper is one of the first to highlight the potential costs of elite conspiracy theory support and complicates popular narratives about QAnon.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data and code needed to replicate the analysis in this manuscript is available on the Political Behaivor Dataverse at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/UVVSIR

Notes

  1. We pre-registered our pre-analysis plans at https://osf.io/6e8az/?view_only=5b20d3be656f47d8922d9e38f6c31684. Studies involving human subjects were IRB-approved.

  2. Mainstream media regularly refers to QAnon as a conspiracy theory, but others, such as the Anti-Defamation League refer to it as a movement. Some point out the QAnon is a “super conspiracy theory” that includes many conspiracy theories under its umbrella. At the time of our data collection, QAnon was often covered as a conspiracy theory. We refer to it as such here.

  3. For more on QAnon, see Uscinski (2022).

  4. For example, COVID-19 (birther) conspiracy theories might be more related to trust in science (partisanship).

  5. Matching covariates include candidate gender, previous officeholder, House or Senate race, whether they advanced to the general election, incumbency, and party. We also match on constituency characteristics including open seat, population density, competitiveness, median age, race, proportion attended college, and median household income.

  6. Our corpus does not include TV-first (e.g., Fox, MSNBC, CNN) or blog-style (e.g., Breitbart, Daily Beast) media. Thus, our sample is biased toward mainstream news, which may lean left. Even so, this sample restriction likely underestimates coverage and negative sentiment as we discuss in Online Appendix A.

  7. We pool both waves but analyze each separately in Online Appendix B.

  8. Balance statistics are provided in Table B1.

  9. We preregistered a second moderator variable, party identification. See Online Appendix B.2.

  10. In wave 1, respondents were asked to “Please read the following article about a hypothetical congressional candidate, which might have appeared in a mainstream newspaper.” In wave 2, respondents were asked to “Please read the following article about a congressional candidate, which recently appeared in a mainstream newspaper:” In Tables B2 and B3, the effects are substantively similar when considering waves separately.

  11. In Wave 1 (2), the candidate’s name was John Smith (Cunningham). Pilot tests on Mechanical Turk did not reveal variation in evaluations based on name.

  12. We framed the candidate as losing because (1) our surveys were fielded after elections; we felt describing the candidate as running for Congress would detract from any realism in our study, and (2) we tried to mirror the reality that many QAnon-supporting candidates lost their elections. In Online Appendix B.5, we discuss results of a follow-up study in which we describe the same candidate as having won the election. The results are similar, and there is no evidence to suggest that QAnon-supporting winners are viewed more favorably.

  13. To ensure our articles were perceived as neutral and negative, we conducted a pilot study on Mechanical Turk on September 24, 2020.

  14. We asked: “How warm or cold do you feel toward the candidate in the article? Ratings between 50 degree(s) and 100 degree(s) mean that you feel favorable and warm toward the candidate. Ratings between 0 and 50 degree(s) mean that you don’t feel favorable toward the candidate and that you don’t care too much for him. You would rate him at the 50 degree(s) mark if you don’t feel particularly warm or cold toward the candidate.” We use a feeling thermometer, rather than vote choice, given a concern about ceiling effects among those with low trust in media—who would potentially vote for candidates in either treatment condition, even if intensity of those preferences differed.

  15. To determine within-treatment differences, we conduct 1000 bootstraps and calculate the 0.996 quantile.

  16. In Online Appendix B, we present results of these regressions controlling for seven-point party identification and seven-point ideology (and other covariates). These effects are not driven by the correlation between trust in media, ideology, and party identification.

  17. We balanced on gender to account for a viral TikTok trend that increased female sign ups on Prolific beginning in summer 2021.

  18. We exclude four independents who took our survey.

  19. We chose to present candidates as either supporting or not supporting (rather than actively opposing) for methodological and substantive reasons that we detail in Online Appendix C.

  20. Comparisons between subgroup AMCEs are sensitive to the reference category (Leeper et al., 2020) however, with the exception of prior experience, our attributes are dichotomous. The AMCEs should not be susceptible to baseline effects (Carey et al., 2022). We present the same plots using marginal means in Online Appendix C. Marginal mean plots are exploratory as they were not pre-reigstered.

References

  • Ahler, D. J., & Sood, G. (2018). The parties in our heads: Misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. The Journal of Politics, 80(3), 964–981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, J. (2023). Awaiting possible indictment, Trump rallies in Waco and vows to “destroy the deep state”. https://www.nbcnews.com/politics/awaiting-possible-indictment-trump-rallies-waco-rcna75684

  • Amsalem, E., Zoizner, A., Sheafer, T., Walgrave, S., & Loewen, P. J. (2020). The effect of politicians’ personality on their media visibility. Communication Research, 47(7), 1079–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., Johnson, M., & Murphy, C. (2012). Polarized political communication, oppositional media hostility, and selective exposure. The Journal of Politics, 74(1), 174–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., & Martin, J. (2013). Changing minds or changing channels? Partisan news in an age of choice. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., & Truex, R. (2022). Donald Trump and the lie. Perspectives on Politics.

  • Carey, J., Clayton, K., Helmke, G., Nyhan, B., Sanders, M., & Stokes, S. (2022). Who will defend democracy? Evaluating tradeoffs in candidate support among partisan donors and voters. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 32(1), 230–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christenson, D. P., Kreps, S. E., & Kriner, D. L. (2020). Contemporary presidency: Going public in an era of social media: Tweets, corrections, and public opinion. Presidential Studies Quarterly.

  • Concepcion, Summer. (2023). QAnon Shaman who stormed the Capitol on Jan. 6 files paperwork to run for Congress. NBC News. Retrieved November 22, 2023, from https://www.nbcnews.com/politics/2024-election/qanon-shaman-stormed-capitol-jan-6-files-paperwork-run-congress-rcna124858

  • Coppock, A. (2023). Persuasion in parallel: How information changes minds about politics. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darr, J. P., Hitt, M. P., & Dunaway, J. L. (2018). Newspaper closures polarize voting behavior. Journal of Communication, 68(6), 1007–028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Benedictis-Kessner, J., Baum, M. A., Berinsky, A. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2019). Persuading the enemy: Estimating the persuasive effects of partisan media with the preference-incorporating choice and assignment design. American Political Science Review, 113(4), 902–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de la, Cuesta, B., Egami, N., & Imai, K. (2021) Improving the external validity of conjoint analysis: The essential role of profile distribution. Political Analysis.

  • Diamond, A., & Sekhon, J. S. (2013). Genetic matching for estimating causal effects: A general multivariate matching method for achieving balance in observational studies. Review of Economics and Statistics, 95(3), 932–945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40(S1), 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egami, N., Green, D., Kim, E., & Mattingly, D. (2023). PICA-2. https://osf.io/sk23a

  • Enders, A. M., & Armaly, M. T. (2019). The differential effects of actual and perceived polarization. Political Behavior, 41(3), 815–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enders, A. M., Uscinski, J. E., Klofstad, C. A., Wuchty, S., Seelig, M. I., Funchion, J. R., Murthi, M. N., Premaratne, K., & Stoler, J. (2022). Who supports QAnon? A case study in political extremism. The Journal of Politics.

  • Garrett, R. K., & Bond, R. M. (2021). Conservatives’ susceptibility to political misperceptions. Science Advances, 7(23), eabf1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gimpel, J. G., Lovin, N., Moy, B., & Reeves, A. (2020). The urban-rural gulf in American political behavior. Political Behavior, 42(4), 1343–1368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guess, A., Nagler, J., & Tucker, J. (2019). Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on Facebook. Science Advances, 5(1), eaau4586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guess, A. M. (2021). (Almost) Everything in moderation: New evidence on Americans’ online media diets. American Journal of Political Science, 65(4), 1007–1022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: Understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis, 22(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, A. B. (2015). What happens when extremists win primaries? American Political Science Review, 109(01), 18–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helfer, L., & Van Aelst, P. (2016). What makes party messages fit for reporting? An experimental study of journalistic news selection. Political Communication, 33(1), 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J., & Reiter, J. P. (2006). Interval estimation for treatment effects using propensity score matching. Statistics in Medicine, 25(13), 2230–2256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2013). Name recognition and candidate support. American Journal of Political Science, 57(4), 971–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, A. (2020). Here are the QAnon supporters running for Congress in 2020. Retrieved January 18, 2023, from https://www.mediamatters.org/qanon-conspiracy-theory/here-are-qanon-supporters-running-congress-2020

  • Lapinski, J., Levendusky, M., Winneg, K., & Jamieson, K. H. (2016). What do citizens want from their member of congress? Political Research Quarterly, 69(3), 535–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T. J., Hobolt, S. B., & Tilley, J. (2020). Measuring subgroup preferences in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis, 28(2), 207–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L., & Farhart, C. E. (2016). Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: The moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. American Journal of Political Science, 60(4), 824–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, Z., Colvin, J., & Seitz, A. (2020). Trump praises QAnon conspiracists, appreciates support. Retrieved January 18, 2023, from https://apnews.com/article/election-2020-ap-top-news-religion-racial-injustice-535e145ee67dd757660157be39d05d3f

  • Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2010). When corrections fail: The persistence of political misperceptions. Political Behavior, 32(2), 303–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 952–966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2020). 5 facts about the QAnon conspiracy theories. Retrieved January 18, 2023, from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/11/16/5-facts-about-the-qanon-conspiracy-theories/

  • Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, R., & Treul, S. A. (2024). Evaluating (in)experience in congressional elections. American Journal of Political Science ajps.12854.

  • PRRI. (2021). Understanding QAnon’s connection to american politics, religion, and media consumption. https://www.prri.org/research/qanon-conspiracy-american-politics-report/

  • Roose, K. (2021). What is QAnon, the viral pro-trump conspiracy theory?. Retrieved November 16, 2023, from https://www.nytimes.com/article/what-is-qanon.html

  • Rubin, O., Bruggeman, L., & Steakin, W. (2021). QAnon emerges as recurring theme of criminal cases tied to US Capitol siege. Retrieved January 18, 2023, from https://abcnews.go.com/US/qanon-emerges-recurring-theme-criminal-cases-tied-us/story?id=75347445

  • Ryan, T. J., & Krupnikov, Y. (2021). Split feelings: Understanding implicit and explicit political persuasion. American Political Science Review 1–18.

  • Stroud, N. J. (2011). Niche news: The politics of news choice. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thorson, E. (2016). Belief echoes: The persistent effects of corrected misinformation. Political Communication, 33(3), 460–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uscinski, J. E. (2018). Conspiracy theories and the people who believe them. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Uscinski, J. E. (2022). Getting QAnon wrong and right. Social Research: An International Quarterly, 89(3), 551–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uscinski, J. E., Enders, A. M., Seelig, M. I., Klofstad, C. A., Funchion, J. R., Everett, C., & Murthi, M. N. (2021). American politics in two dimensions: Partisan and ideological identities versus anti-establishment orientations. American Journal of Political Science .

  • Uscinski, J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, V., Carey, J., Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2022). Legislator criticism of a candidate’s conspiracy beliefs reduces support for the conspiracy but not the candidate: Evidence from Marjorie Taylor Greene and QAnon. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review.

Download references

Funding

The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benjamin S. Noble.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Ethical Approval

Taylor Carlson is a member of the Political Behavior Editorial Board. The questionnaires and survey methodology for this study were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Washington University in St. Louis.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

We thank the Weidenbaum Center on the Economy, Government, and Public Policy for providing support for this project through The American Social Survey (TASS). We thank Nick Anspach, Ted Enamorado, Hans Hassell, Jonathan Green, Christopher Lucas, Jacob Montgomery, Will Nomikos, Mike Olson, Keith Schnakenberg, Betsy Sinclair, and Carly Wayne for helpful feedback on this manuscript. We thank audiences at MPSA 2021 and Florida State University for feedback. We thank Maggie O’Connor, Taylor Degitz, and Oliver Rosand for research assistance. The pre-analysis plan for the experiments is available at: https://osf.io/6e8az/?view_only=5b20d3be656f47d8922d9e38f6c31684.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file 1 (pdf 432 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Noble, B.S., Carlson, T.N. CueAnon: What QAnon Signals About Congressional Candidates and What it Costs Them. Polit Behav (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-024-09957-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-024-09957-3

Keywords

Navigation