Skip to main content
Log in

Moral Rhetoric, Extreme Positions, and Perceptions of Candidate Sincerity

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most Americans believe that politicians do not try to keep their campaign promises. This deep level of cynicism threatens to break a fundamental link in representation and undermines the legislative process. If candidates cannot credibly convey their positions, then voters will not trust them to enact policies. Yet, we know little about the strategies politicians might take to convey the sincerity of their claims. We argue that politicians can signal sincerity by justifying their stances in moral terms or by taking more extreme positions. Across three experiments, our results suggest that moral justifications tend to enhance perceived sincerity, while extreme positions do not. In a fourth study, we show that extreme stances increase polarization in candidate evaluations, but moral justifications do not. Taken together, our findings suggest that moral justifications are a useful strategy to reduce cynicism without contributing to rising levels of polarization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Replication data is available at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AKKDGC.

  2. https://forwardfla.com/our-work/.

  3. https://www.cnn.com/2018/10/14/politics/immigration-campaign-ads-midterms/index.html.

  4. This statement is consistent with a growing body of work showing that the relative success of clarity vs. ambiguity is dependent upon a number of conditions (e,g, Milita et al. 2017; Simas 2021a; Simas, Milita, and Ryan 2021b).

  5. Kreps, Laurin, and Merritt (2017) do advance a partisanship hypothesis, but they focus on agreement with the second of two conflicting stances taken by the speaker, not shared party identity.

  6. Again, we find no evidence of an interaction, as the effects are similar for the extreme pragmatic condition (b = − 0.15, p = .005) and the extreme moral condition (b = − 0.22, p = .001).

  7. The weighted AAPOR RR3 cumulative response rate was 6.3%. This study was funded and fielded by Time-Sharing Experiments for the Social Sciences (TESS). Study 4 was also funded by TESS.

  8. Across the three candidate trials, the Cronbach’s alpha ranges from 0.78 to 0.79.

  9. Specifically, they are identical in sign and significance, and highly similar in effect size. We maintain random effects to be consistent with models that incorporate individual differences (e.g., partisan identity).

  10. We find no evidence of interaction effects between the treatments for either outcome (ps > 0.59).

  11. We also explored whether the results are consistent across issues and find no evidence that the results are driven by a specific issue.

  12. Formerly the Cooperative Congressional Election Study. Principle Investigators Brian Schaffner, Stephen Ansolabehere, and Sam Luks. For more information, see https://cces.gov.harvard.edu/.

  13. We measured several character traits, but these traits loaded on multiple factors, so we focus on the “moral” trait as the most relevant. We get substantively identical results when instead analyzing “compassionate.”

  14. For details, see the Appendix.

  15. https://www.langerresearch.com/wp-content/uploads/1207a2GunPolicy.pdf.

References

  • Abramowitz, A. I., and Steven Webster (2016). The rise of negative partisanship and the nationalization of U.S. Elections in the 21st Century. Electoral Studies, 41, 12–22.

  • Alvarez, R., & Michael, and Charles H. Franklin (1994). Uncertainty and political perceptions. The Journal of Politics, 56(3), 671–688.

  • Aragones, E., and Zvika Neeman (2000). Strategic Ambiguity in Electoral Competition. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 12(2), 183–204.

  • Aramovich, N. P., Brad, L., & Lytle, and Linda J. Skitka (2012). Opposing torture: Moral conviction and resistance to Majority Influence. Social Influence, 7(1), 21–34.

  • Berinsky, A. J. (2017). Rumors and Health Care Reform: experiments in political misinformation. British Journal of Political Science, 47(02), 241–262.

  • Born, A., van Eck, P., & Johannesson, M. (2018). An experimental investigation of election promises. Political Psychology, 39(3), 685–705.

  • Brady, D. W., & Han, H., and Jeremy C. Pope (2007). Primary elections and candidate ideology: out of step with the primary electorate? Legislative Studies Quarterly, 32(1), 79–105.

  • Broockman, D. E., & Carnes, N., Melody Crowder-Meyer, and Christopher Skovron (2021). Why local Party leaders don’t support nominating centrists. British Journal of Political Science, 51(2), 724–749.

  • Chanley, V. A., Thomas, J., Rudolph, & Rahn, W. M. (2000). The Origins and Consequences of Public Trust in Government: a Time Series Analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 64(3), 239–256.

  • Citrin, J. (2018). and Laura Stoker. “Political Trust in a Cynical Age.” https://www.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-050316-09255021:49–70

  • Ciuk, D. J. (2020). and Joshua Rottman. “Moral Conviction, Emotion, and the Influence of Episodic versus Thematic Frames.”Political Communication.

  • Clifford, S. (2014). Linking issue stances and trait inferences: a theory of Moral Exemplification. The Journal of Politics, 76(03), 698–710.

  • Clifford, S. (2019). How emotional frames moralize and polarize political attitudes. Political Psychology, 40(1), 75–91.

  • Clifford, S. (2020). Compassionate Democrats and tough Republicans: how ideology shapes partisan stereotypes. Political Behavior, 42(4), 1269–1293.

  • Clifford, S. (2022). How Moral Motives Link Party Stereotypes. Political Behavior, 44, 705–724.

  • Clifford, S., Geoffrey Sheagley, and Spencer Piston (2021). Increasing Precision without Altering Treatment Effects: repeated measures designs in Survey experiments. American Political Science Review, 115(3), 1048–1065.

  • Cormack, L. (2021). and Kristyn L. Karl. “Why Women Earn High Marks: Examining the Role of Partisanship and Gender in Political Evaluations.”Politics & Gender:1–30.

  • Costa, M. (2021). “Ideology, Not Affect: What Americans Want from Political Representation.”American Journal of Political Science.

  • Delton, A. W., Peter, DeScioli, & Timothy, J. R. (2020). Moral Obstinacy in political negotiations. Political Psychology, 41(1), 3–20.

  • Doherty, D. (2015). Perceived Motives in the political Arena. American Politics Research, 43(3), 363–393.

  • Druckman, J. N., Thomas, J., & Leeper (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: pretreatment and its Effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.

  • Effron, D., & Dale, T. M. (2012). How the moralization of Issues Grants Social Legitimacy to Act on One’s attitudes. Personality & social psychology bulletin, 38(5), 690–701.

  • Fairbrother, M. (2019). When Will People pay to pollute? Environmental taxes, Political Trust and experimental evidence from Britain. British Journal of Political Science, 49(2), 661–682.

  • Fenno, R. F. (1978). Home Style: House Members in their districts. New York: Longman.

  • Fernandez-Vazquez, P. (2018a). The credibility of Party Policy Rhetoric Survey Experimental evidence. The Journal of Politics, 81(1), 309–314.

  • Fernandez-Vazquez, P. (2018b). “Voter Discounting of Party Campaign Manifestos: An Analysis of Mainstream and Niche Parties in Western Europe, 1971–2011.” Party Politics.

  • Garrett, K. N. (2019). Fired up by morality: the unique physiological response tied to Moral conviction in politics. Political Psychology, 40(3), 543–563.

  • Garrett, K. N. (2018). and Alexa Bankert. “The Moral Roots of Partisan Division: How Moral Conviction Heightens Affective Polarization.”British Journal of Political Science:1–20.

  • Glasgow, G., and R. Michael Alvarez (2000). Uncertainty and candidate personality traits. American Politics Quarterly, 28(1), 26–49.

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Brian a Nosek (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of Moral Foundations. Journal of personality and social psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.

  • Groseclose, T. (2001). A model of candidate location when one candidate has a Valence advantage. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 862.

  • Hetherington, M. J., and Suzanne Globetti (2002). Political Trust and racial policy preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 46(2), 253.

  • Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: polarization, Political Trust, and the governing Crisis. University of Chicago Press.

  • Hibbing, J., and Elizabeth Theiss-Morse (2002). Stealth Democracy: Americans’ beliefs about how Government should work. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Jacobs, L. R., Robert, Y., & Shapiro (2000). Politicians don’t Pander: political manipulation and the loss of democratic responsiveness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • Jung, J. H. (2020). The mobilizing effect of parties’ Moral Rhetoric. American Journal of Political Science, 64(2), 341–355.

  • Just, M. R., et al. (1996). Crosstalk: citizens, candidates, and the media in a presidential campaign. University of Chicago Press.

  • Kahn, K. F., & Kenney, P. J. (1999). The spectacle of U.S. Senate Campaigns. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Kennedy, R., et al. (2020). The shape of and solutions to the MTurk Quality Crisis. Political Science Research and Methods, 8(4), 614–629.

  • Kodapanakkal, R. I., Mark, J., Brandt, C., Kogler, Ilja, & van Beest (2022). “Moral Frames Persuade and Moralize, Non-Moral Frames Persuade and Demoralize.”Psychological Science.

  • Kreps, T. A., Laurin, K., & Merritt, A. C. (2017). Hypocritical Flip-Flop, or courageous evolution? When leaders change their Moral minds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(5), 730–752.

  • Kreps, T. A., & Benoît, M. (2014). Core values Versus Common sense: consequentialist views appear less rooted in morality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(11), 1529–1542.

  • Luttrell, A. (2020). and LaCount J. Togans. “The Stability of Moralized Attitudes Over Time.”Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

  • McGraw, K. M., & Lodge, M., and Jeffrey M. Jones (2002). The pandering politicians of suspicious minds. Journal of Politics, 64(2), 362–383.

  • Milita, K., Ryan, J. B., & Simas, E. N. (2014). “Nothing to Hide, Nowhere to Run, or Nothing to Lose: Candidate Position-Taking in Congressional Elections.”Political Behavior36(2).

  • Milita, K., Elizabeth, N. E. N., Simas, J., Barry, J. B., & Ryan, and Yanna Krupnikov (2017). The Effects of ambiguous rhetoric in Congressional Elections. Electoral Studies, 46, 48–63.

  • Mummolo, J., and Erik Peterson (2019). Demand Effects in Survey experiments: an empirical Assessment. American Political Science Review, 113(2), 517–529.

  • Peterson, D. A. M. (2004). Certainty or accessibility: attitude strength in candidate evaluations. American Journal of Political Science, 48(3), 513–520.

  • Peterson, D. A. M. (2005). Heterogeneity and certainty in candidate evaluations. Political Behavior, 27(1), 1–24.

  • Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (1995). Attitude strength: antecedents and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

  • Rapoport, R. B., Kelly, L., Metcalf, & Hartman, J. A. (1989). Candidate traits and Voter Inferences: an experimental study. Journal of Politics, 51(4), 917–932.

  • Rogowski, J. C., & Sutherland, J. L. (2016). How ideology fuels affective polarization. Political Behavior, 38(2), 485–508.

  • Rudolph, T. J. (2009). Political Trust, ideology, and Public Support for Tax Cuts. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(1), 144–158.

  • Rudolph, T. J., and Jillian Evans (2005). Political Trust, ideology, and public support for government spending. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 660–671.

  • Ryan, T. J. (2014). “Reconsidering Moral Issues in Politics.”The Journal of Politics:1–18.

  • Ryan, T. J. (2017). No compromise: political Consequences of Moralized Attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 61(2), 409–423.

  • Ryan, T. J. (2019). Actions Versus Consequences in Political Arguments: insights from Moral psychology. Journal of Politics, 81(2), 426–440.

  • Shepsle, K. A. (1972). The strategy of ambiguity: uncertainty and Electoral Competition. The American Political Science Review, 66(2), 555–568.

  • Simas, E. N. (2021a). “Medicare for All, Some, or None? Testing the Effects of Ambiguity in the Context of the 2020 Presidential Election.”PS - Political Science and Politics.

  • Simas, E. N., & Milita, K., and John Barry Ryan (2021a). Ambiguous rhetoric and legislative accountability. Journal of Politics, 83(4), 1695–1705.

  • Simas, E. N. (2021b). Extremely high Quality?How ideology shapes perceptions of candidates’ personal traits. Public Opinion Quarterly, 84(3), 699–724.

  • Skitka, L. J. (2010). The psychology of Moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(4), 267–281.

  • Skitka, L. J., Brittany, E., Hanson, G., Scott Morgan, & Wisneski, D. C. (2021). The psychology of Moral conviction. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(5), 347–366.

  • Skitka, L. J., & Scott G. Morgan. (2014). The Social and Political Implications of Moral conviction. Political Psychology, 35, 95–110.

  • Skitka, L. J., Christopher W., Bauman, & Edward, G. Sargis. (2005). “Moral conviction: another contributor to attitude strength or something more? Journal of personality and social psychology, 88(6), 895–917.

  • Slothuus, R. (2016). Assessing the influence of political parties on Public Opinion: the challenge from pretreatment Effects. Political Communication, 33(2), 302–327.

  • Somer-Topcu, Z. (2015). Everything to everyone: the Electoral Consequences of the broad-appeal strategy in Europe. American Journal of Political Science, 59(4), 841–854.

  • Stiers, D., et al. (2019). “Candidate Authenticity: ‘To Thine Own Self Be True.’”Political Behavior.

  • Stone, W. J., & Simas, E. N. (2010). Candidate Valence and ideological positions in U.S. House Elections. American Journal of Political Science, 54(2), 371–388.

  • Sulkin, T. (2009). “Campaign Appeals and Legislative Action.”Journal of Politics.

  • Sulkin, T. (2011). The legislative legacy of Congressional Campaigns. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Tavits, M. (2007). Principle vs. pragmatism: policy shifts and political competition. American Journal of Political Science, 51(1), 151–165.

  • Thomson, R., et al. (2017). The fulfillment of parties’ Election Pledges: a comparative study on the impact of power sharing. American Journal of Political Science, 61(3), 527–542.

  • Webster, S. W. and Alan I. Abramowitz (2017). “The Ideological Foundations of Affective Polarization in the U.S. Electorate.”American Politics Research:1532673X1770313.

  • Zlatev, J. J. (2019). “I May Not Agree With You, but I Trust You: Caring About Social Issues Signals Integrity.”Psychological Science.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott Clifford.

Ethics declarations

Statement on Ethics

All studies reported in this manuscript were reviewed and approved by the University of Houston Institutional Review Board.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Clifford, S., Simas, E.N. Moral Rhetoric, Extreme Positions, and Perceptions of Candidate Sincerity. Polit Behav 46, 523–542 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09835-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09835-w

Navigation