Abstract
Politicians use political value rhetoric to win elections or persuade constituents towards policy positions, but the effectiveness of this rhetoric is unclear. I argue that partisan forces constrain the effectiveness of this rhetoric and that this constraint is conditional based on the value evoked and the match between the politician’s and message recipient’s partisanship. To examine this, I conduct a survey experiment with a diverse U.S. national population and show that politicians’ value rhetoric is disproportionately evaluated based on the value evoked as well as whether the politician is in-party or out-party: in-party politicians are punished—and out-party politicians rewarded—for trespassing on the other party’s values. I then use individual-level variables to examine what drives this result, finding that both party-congruent value endorsements and affective polarization levels moderate the asymmetric responses to political value trespassing. Lastly, I replicate this experiment and reproduce the same findings. The results speak to political values, the effectiveness of political rhetoric, party betrayal signaling, and the true object of out-party distaste, which seems to be more about the party than the party member.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Political values are related to, but distinct from, human values, where the latter are broader, more durable, and perhaps even shape the former (see Schwartz et al. 2014 for discussion). Because of these differences, it is unclear if findings here on political values would replicate with human values.
When evoking the expected values, the partisanship of the speaker did not predict attitudes or behavioral intentions (see Nelson and Garst 2005, pg. 506).
Dynata is an online market research firm that recruits participants to take surveys for compensation and prides itself on data quality (see https://www.dynata.com/dynatas-world-class-quality/). See Appendices E and F for demographic comparisons to nationally-representative data.
This was done using block randomization by party.
Because gender could matter here (see, e.g., Bauer 2019; Herrnson, Lay, and Stokes 2003), I kept gender constant as male.
This design used deception in that respondents were told about a candidate and slogan that did not exist. This deception was low risk and was necessary in order to have control over various factors in the study. Participants (in both studies) were debriefed at the end of the survey.
See Appendix E for details.
Either because of deeply held beliefs (e.g., Lupton and McKee 2020) or social influence (Connors 2020).
16 respondents did not answer questions about affective polarization and/or value endorsements and were thus dropped from this analysis. 52 respondents did not answer questions about age or interest—I imputed these responses using multiple imputation.
The value endorsement questions had four response options, but integrating whether endorsement was party-congruent or conflicting leads to eight values—where 8 represents a partisan endorsing a party-congruent value to the greatest extent and 1 represents a partisan endorsing a party-conflicting value to the greatest extent.
This interaction is significant when run without the other two interactions but with the same controls (α=-1.82, p = .000), suggesting multicollinearity. Nonetheless, even if standard errors are inflated due to multicollinearity, this interaction’s coefficient is rather small—especially in comparison to the other two. Either way, I urge caution in interpreting this null result.
Because this variation (an added cue) did not significantly change the means of the conditions, I group those without and those with the cue into the same condition. Full survey in Appendix F.
40 respondents did not answer questions about ideology or affective polarization and were dropped from this analysis. Two respondents did not answer questions about age—I imputed these responses using multiple imputation.
References
Abramowitz, A. I., & Webster, S. (2016). The Rise of Negative Partisanship and the Nationalization of U.S. Elections in the 21st Century. Electoral Studies, 41, 12–22
Abramowitz, A. I., & Webster, S. (2018). Negative Partisanship: Why Americans Dislike Parties but Behave like Rabid Partisans. Advances in Political Psychology, 38, 119–135
Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1994). Riding the Wave and Claiming Ownership Over Issues: The Joint Effects of Advertising and News Coverage in Campaigns. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 58(3), 335–357
Arceneaux, K. (2008). Can Partisan Cues Diminish Democratic Accountability? Political Behavior, 30(2), 139–160
Bartels, L. M. (2000). Partisanship and Voting Behavior, 1952–1996. American Journal of Political Science:35–50
Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150
Bauer, N. M. (2019). The Effects of Partisan Trespassing Strategies Across Candidate Sex. Political Behavior, 41, 897–915
Belander, E., & Bonnie, M. M. (2008). Issue Salience, Issue Ownership, and Issue-Based Vote Choice. Electoral Studies, 27(3), 477–491
Bellucci, P. (2006). Tracing the Cognitive and Affective Roots of ‘Party Competence’: Italy and Britain, 2001. Electoral Studies, 25(3), 548–569
Bullock, J. G. (2009). Partisan Bias and the Bayesian Ideal in the Study of Public Opinion. The Journal of Politics, 71(3), 1109–1124
Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American Voter. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press
Caprara, G. V. & Vecchione, M. (2013). Personality Approaches to Political Behavior. In The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, edited by Leonie Huddy, David O. Sears, and Jack S. Levy. Oxford University Press
Ciuk, D. J. (2016). Americans’ Value Preferences Pre- and Post-9/11. Social Science Quarterly, 97, 407–417
Ciuk, D. J. (2017). Democratic Values? A Racial Group-Based Analysis of Core Political Values, Partisanship, and Ideology. Political Behavior, 39, 479–501
Ciuk, D. J., & Jacoby, W. G. (2015). Checking for Systematic Value Preferences Using the Method of Triads. Political Psychology, 36(6), 709–728
Ciuk, D. J., Lupton, R. N., & Thornton, J. R. (2018). Value Voters: The Conditional Effect of Income on the Relationship between Core Values and Political Attitudes and Behavior. Political Psychology, 39, 869–888
Clarke, H. D. (2004). Political Choice in Britain. Oxford University Press on Demand
Clifford, S., & Jerit, J. (2013). How Words Do the Work of Politics: Moral Foundations Theory and the Debate over Stem Cell Research. Journal of Politics, 75(3), 659–671
Clifford, S., Jerit, J., Rainey, C., & Motyl, M. (2015). Moral Concerns and Policy Attitudes: Investigating the Influence of Elite Rhetoric. Political Communication, 33(2), 229–248
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party Over Policy: The Dominating Impact of Group Influence on Political Beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808–822
Condor, S., & Tileagă, C. (2013). and Michael Billig. Political Rhetoric. In The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, edited by Leonie Huddy, David O. Sears, and Jack S. Levy. Oxford University Press
Connors, E. C. (2020). The Social Dimension of Political Values. Political Behavior, 42, 961–982
Day, M. V., Fiske, S. T., Downing, E. L., & Trail, T. E. (2014). Shifting Liberal and Conservative Attitudes Using Moral Foundations Theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12), 1559–1573
DeMora, S. L., Merolla, J. L., & Newman, B. (2021). and Elizabeth J. Zechmeister. Reducing Mask Resistance Among White Evangelical Christians with Value-Consistent Messages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118(21):1–3
Druckman, J. N. (2001). On the Limits of Framing Effects: Who Can Frame? The Journal of Politics, 63(4), 1041–1066
Druckman, J. N., & Levendusky, M. S. (2019). What Do We Measure When We Measure Affective Polarization? Public Opinion Quarterly, 83(1), 114–122
Egan, P. J. (2013). Partisan Priorities: How Issue Ownership Drives and Distorts American Politics. Cambridge University Press
Enders, A. M., & Lupton, R. N. (2020). Value Extremity Contributes to Affective Polarization in the US. Political Science Research and Methods:1–10
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2015). From Gulf to Bridge: When Do Moral Arguments Facilitate Political Influence? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(12), 1665–1681
Feldman, S., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2001). The Humanitarian Foundation of Public Support for Social Welfare. American Journal of Political Science, 45(3), 658–677
Feldman, S. (1988). Structure and Consistency in Public Opinion: The Role of Core Beliefs and Values. American Journal of Political Science:416–440
Feldman, S., & Zaller, J. (1992). The Political Culture of Ambivalence: Ideological Responses to the Welfare State. American Journal of Political Science, 36(1), 268–307
Feldman, S. (2013). Political Ideology. In The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, edited by Leonie Huddy, David O. Sears, and Jack S. Levy. Oxford University Press
Goren, P., Federico, C. M., & Miki, C. K. (2009). Source Cues, Partisan Identities, and Political Value Expression. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 805–820
Goren, P. (2005). Party Identification and Core Political Values. American Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 881–896
Green, D., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan Hearts and Minds: Political Parties and the Social Identities of Voters. Yale University Press
Hayes, D. (2005). Candidate Qualities through a Partisan Lens: A Theory of Trait Ownership. American Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 908–923
Hayes, D. (2011). When Gender and Party Collide: Stereotyping in Candidate Trait Attribution. Politics & Gender, 7, 133–165
Herrnson, P. S., Celeste Lay, J., & Atiya, K. S. (2003). Women Running ‘As Women’: Candidate Gender, Campaign Issues, and Voter-Targeting Strategies. The Journal of Politics, 65(1), 244–255
Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N., & Sean, J. W. (2019). The Origins and Consequences of Affective Polarization in the United States. Annual Review of Political Science, 22, 129–146
Jerit, J. (2008). Issue Framing and Engagement: Rhetorical Strategy in Public Policy Debates. Political Behavior, 30(1), 1–24
Kam, C. D. (2005). Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and Individual Differences. Political Behavior, 27(2), 163–182
Kam, C. D., & Trussler, M. J. (2017). At the Nexus of Experimental and Observational Research: Theory, Specification, and Analysis of Experiments with Heterogeneous Treatment Effects. Political Behavior, 39(4), 789–815
Kane, J. V. (2019a). Fight Clubs: Media Coverage of Party (Dis)unity and Citizens’ Selective Exposure to It. Political Research Quarterly
Kane, J. V. (2019b). Enemy or Ally? Elites, Base Relations, and Partisanship in America. Public Opinion Quarterly, 83(3), 534–558
Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by Color: Racial Politics and Democratic Ideals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Kingzette, J. (2021). Who Do You Loathe? Feelings toward Politicians vs. Ordinary People in the Opposing Party. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 8, 75–84
Klar, S., & Leeper, T. (2019). and Joshua Robison. Studying Identities with Experiments: Weighing the Risk of Posttreatment Bias Against Priming Effects. Journal of Experimental Political Science:1–5
Lupton, R. N., Smallpage, S. M., &, Enders, A. M. (2020). Values and Political Predispositions in the Age of Polarization: Examining the Relationship Between Partisanship and Ideology in the United States, 1988–2012. British Journal of Political Science, 50(10, 241–260
Lupton, R. N., & Seth, C. M. (2020). Dixie’s Drivers: Core Values and the Southern Republican Realignment. Journal of Politics, 82(3), 921–936
Mason, L. (2015). ‘I Disrespectfully Agree’: The Differential Effects of Partisan Sorting on Social and Issue Polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 128–145
Nadeau, R., Blais, A., Gidengil, E., & Nevitte, N. (2001). Perceptions of Party Competence in the 1997 Election. Party Politics in Canada, 8, 413–430
Nelson, T. E., & Garst, J. (2005). Values-Based Political Messages and Persuasion: Relationships among Speaker, Recipient, and Evoked Values. Political Psychology, 26(4), 489–515
Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997a). Media Framing of a Civil Liberties Conflict and Its Effect on Tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91(3), 567–583
Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997b). Toward a Psychology of Framing Effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246
Norpoth, H., & Buchanan, B. (1992). Wanted: The Education President Issue Trespassing by Political Candidates. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56(1), 87–99
Peffley, M., Knigge, P., & Hurwitz, J. (2001). A Multiple Values Model of Political Tolerance. Political Research Quarterly, 54, 379–406
Petrocik, J. R. (1996). Issue Ownership in the Presidential Elections, with a 1980 Case Study. American Journal of Political Science, 40(3), 825–850
Rahn, W. (1993). The Role of Partisan Stereotypes in Information Processing about Political Candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472–496
Riker, W. H. (1996). The Strategy of Rhetoric: Campaigning for the American Constitution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Schwartz, S. H., et al. (2014). Basic Personal Values Underlie and Give Coherence to Political Values: A Cross National Study in 15 Countries. Political Behavior, 36, 899–930
Snyder, J. M. Jr., & Ting, M. M. (2002). An Informational Rationale for Political Parties. American Journal of Political Science, 46(1), 90–110
Thesen, G., Green-Pedersen, C., & Peter, B. M. (2016). Priming, Issue Ownership, and Party Support: The Electoral Gains of an Issue-Friendly Media Agenda. Political Communication:1–20
Van der Brug, W. (2004). Issue Ownership and Party Choice. Electoral Studies, 23(2), 209–233
Voelkel, J. G. & Feinberg, M. (2018). Morally Reframed Arguments Can Affect Support for Political Candidates. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(8), 917–924
Acknowledgements
Previous versions of this paper have received valuable feedback from Yanna Krupnikov, Jennifer Jerit, Peter DeScioli, Vin Arceneaux, David Darmofal, and extremely helpful anonymous reviewers.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Statements and Declarations:
Research was conducted under the approval of Stony Brook University Institutional Review Board and was funded internally by Stony Brook University’s political science department. It was not pre-registered. Replication data for this paper can be found at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U5GT7X.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Connors, E.C. Party Foul: The Effectiveness of Political Value Rhetoric is Constrained by Party Ownership. Polit Behav 46, 707–726 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09821-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09821-2