Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Legislative vs. Executive Political Offices: How Gender Stereotypes Can Disadvantage Women in Either Office

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Different stereotypes exist for women politicians in a way that is not true for men; a difference that may affect voter evaluations. While some research concludes that these stereotypes disadvantage women candidates, other findings suggest there is no effect, or that women may have some advantages. I employ three unique experiments to offer insight to this disconnect, expanding the knowledge of stereotypes about the goals of legislative vs. executive offices and female politicians in these roles. This analysis provides insight into how the type of office matters, sharpening our understanding of gender stereotypes in candidate evaluation. I find that stereotypes of women in these offices are unique from those of an ungendered officeholder or male officeholders and stereotypes of women are vastly positive, but that these positive stereotypes never equate to an electoral feminine advantage and may penalize a woman with traits counter to the stereotype of the office.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A large and conflicted literature exists about whether and when stereotypes matter to voters. For the purposes of this paper, I assume that—at least in some conditions and in some ways—the existence of stereotypes may matter to voters (see Bauer, 2017).

  2. Fox and Oxley (2003) classify feminine offices as those that deal explicitly with a traditional feminine policy area like education.

  3. Reliance on convenience samples, as MTurk generates, has the potential to inject bias into a study as samples are unrepresentative and tend to skew more educated and more liberal—as is the case in all three studies described in this manuscript. It should be noted that while this bias exists, a more educated and liberal sample is actually a more robust test of whether these stereotypes exist since liberals (Sanbonmatsu & Dolan, 2009) and those with higher levels of education have been shown to be less prone to stereotype reliance (Bauer, 2015).

  4. The prompt, following Schneider and Bos (2014) read, “Society is composed of many different groups about whom people in general have some knowledge. In fact, the ease with which people form relatively well-defined impressions about the individuals and social groups that surround them greatly simplifies their social life. On many occasions, either through hearsay or direct contact, we find out something about the impressions that people in general have about social groups. In this study, you will be asked to give your opinion about what people in general think about some social groups. Naturally, the impressions that people in general have about social groups may or may not reflect your personal beliefs. So give your answers based on what you know to be the culturally shared beliefs people in general have about those social groups, whether or not you believe those ideas to be true.”.

  5. For example, “educated” and “well-educated” were both prevalent responses across conditions and would have both met the standard for inclusion in the master list. These were combined into “well-educated” to avoid unnecessary repetition in the list. Synonyms like “arrogant” and “stuck-up” were combined into the more concise arrogant.

  6. Augmentation of the trait list using those that appeared in previous research follows the approach utilized by Schneider and Bos (2014) in their examination of the female politician subtype.

  7. Recent analysis of the electorate would suggest the Democratic skew of this sample is not inconsistent with party affiliation in the general population, https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/10/26/what-the-2020-electorate-looks-like-by-party-race-and-ethnicity-age-education-and-religion/.

  8. In both study 1 and 2 participants were given examples of each type of office at each level of office to avoid any misunderstanding of what constituted a legislative vs. executive office.

  9. Inter-coder reliability was .864 (Cohen’s Kappa) across two independent coders. Disagreement was resolved by a third independent coder.

  10. Not all of the adjectives (have an available ranking in this dataset. The adjective is rated by the researcher when it is otherwise unavailable. One trait, wealthy, was coded as negative despite a positive valence in ANEW because the wealth is generally viewed negatively in a political context.

  11. As in Bauer (2019), these vignettes used photos and names of two candidates Mary/Mark Buckley. Bauer pre-tested the names and photos separately and found no statistically significant differences in average evaluation of age, education, or attractiveness.

  12. These three measures notably each have their flaws in candidate evaluation. Vote choice potentially lacks realism in an experimental setting that offers no choice of candidates (Brooks, 2013) and could provoke social desirability biases that inflate support for the female candidate (Krupnikov et al., 2016). Viability assessments may avoid these pitfalls but have been noted as using an implicitly gendered standard in an implicitly gendered environment to prompt participant assessment (Claassen & Ryan, 2016).

  13. Only traits chosen, on average, by 30% or more of respondents across all office types are included in this analysis.

  14. Cronbach’s Alpha .809.

  15. Cronbach’s Alpha .902.

  16. Across all conditions, the Communal Ratio ranged from 0 to .94 and the Agentic Ratio ranged from 0 to .96, with communal having a mean of .09 and agentic having a mean .31.

  17. The few traits on which the generic officeholder and male officeholders differ produce more positive stereotypes for the generic officeholder in the comparison which, in turn, makes a comparison between a female officeholder and a generic officeholder (Table 3) a more robust test of the existence of a female subtype. See Online Appendix B for comparisons of the generic and male officeholders.

  18. Analyses are limited to just the most frequently attributed traits to make the analysis and corresponding narrative based on only those traits most frequently identified. Additional comparisons between conditions are available upon request.

  19. Bauer (2019) notes, and it applies equally here that a social desirability bias may contribute to the more positive attributions women receive (also see Burden et al., 2017; Claassen & Ryan, 2016; Krupnikov et al., 2016; Streb et al., 2008). The methodology used in this study, however, allowed for participants to attribute as many traits from the list as they desired, so attribution was not a zero-sum game. I suspect this makes it less likely that women were over-attributed or men under-attributed.

  20. There also exists an abundance of scholarship reinforcing the idea that women are more effective lawmakers (Volden & Wiseman, 2013; Lazarus & Steigerwalt, 2018; Sweet-Cushman, 2020).

References

  • Albritton, M. D., Oswald, S. L., & Anderson, J. S. (2008). Leadership quality and follower affect: A study of US presidential candidates. Journal of Leadership Studies, 1(4), 6–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, N. M. (2015). Emotional, sensitive, and unfit for office? Gender stereotype activation and support female candidates. Political Psychology, 36(6), 691–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, N. M. (2017). The effects of counterstereotypic gender strategies on candidate evaluations. Political Psychology, 38(2), 279–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, N. M. (2018). Running local: Gender stereotyping and female candidates in local elections. Urban Affairs Review 1078087418770807.

  • Bauer, N. M. (2019). A feminine advantage? Delineating the effects of feminine trait and feminine issue messages on evaluations of female candidates. Politics & Gender 1–21.

  • Bauer, N. M. (2020a). Shifting standards: How voters evaluate the qualifications of female and male candidates. The Journal of Politics, 82(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, N. M. (2020b). The qualifications gap: Why women must be better than men to win political office. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. (Vol. 30, no. 1.) The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

  • Brooks, D. J. (2013). He runs, she runs: Why gender stereotypes do not harm women candidates. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burden, B. C., Ono, Y., & Yamada, M. (2017). Reassessing public support for a female president. The Journal of Politics, 79(3), 1073–1078.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt-Way, B., & Kelly, R. M. (1992). Gender and sustaining political ambition: A study of Arizona elected officials. Western Political Quarterly, 45(1), 11–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, D., & Preece, J. R. (2016). Recruitment and perceptions of gender bias in party leader support. Political Research Quarterly, 69(4), 842–851.

    Google Scholar 

  • Center for American Women in Politics. Current facts. Retrieved from http://www.cawp.rutgers.edu/current-numbers.

  • Claassen, R. L., & Ryan, J. B. (2016). Social desirability, hidden biases, and support for Hillary Clinton. PS: Political Science & Politics, 49(4), 730–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conroy, M. (2016). Masculinity, media, and the American presidency. Springer

  • Crowder-Meyer, M., Gadarian, S. K., & Trounstine, J. (2015). Electoral institutions, gender stereotypes, and women’s local representation. Politics, Groups, and Identities, 3(2), 318–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diekman, A. B., Brown, E. R., Johnston, A. M., & Clark, E. K. (2010). Seeking congruity between goals and roles: A new look at why women opt out of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics careers. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1051–1057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diekman, A., & Steinberg, M. (2013). Navigating social roles in pursuit of important goals: A communal goal congruity account of STEM pursuits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(7), 487–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ditonto, T. M., Hamilton, A. J., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2014). Gender stereotypes, information search, and voting behavior in political campaigns. Political Behavior, 36(2), 335–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodd, L., & Oppenheimer, B. (2012). Congress reconsidered. Cq Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, D., Dowling, C., & Miller, M. (2019). Do local party chairs think women and minority candidates can win? Evidence from a conjoint experiment. The Journal of Politics, 81(4), 1282–1297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K. (2014). When does gender matter?: Women candidates and gender stereotypes in American elections. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K., & Lynch, T. (2016). The impact of gender stereotypes on voting for women candidates by level and type of office. Politics & Gender, 12(3), 573–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. (2007). Female leadership advantage and disadvantage: Resolving the contradictions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3), 573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A., & Wood, W. (2011). Social role theory. Handbook of Theories in Social Psychology, 2, 458–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A., Nater, C., Miller, D., Kaufmann, M., & Sczesny, S. (2019). Gender stereotypes have changed: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of US public opinion polls from 1946 to 2018. American Psychologist, 75(3), 301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenno, R. (1978). Home style: House members in their districts. HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, L., & McClure, R. (1990). Who decides to run for Congress. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R., & Oxley, Z. (2003). Gender Stereotyping In State Executive Elections: Candidate selection and success. The Journal of Politics, 65(3), 833–850. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-2508.00214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fridkin, K., & Kenney, P. (2011). The role of candidate traits in campaigns. The Journal of Politics, 73(1), 61–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulton, S. (2014). When gender matters: Macro-dynamics and micro-mechanisms. Political Behavior, 36(3), 605–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulton, S., Maestas, C., Maisel, S., & Stone, W. (2006). The sense of a woman: Gender, ambition, and the decision to run for congress. Political Research Quarterly, 59(2), 235–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenlee, J., Holman, M., & VanSickle-Ward, R. (2014). Making it personal: Assessing the impact of in-class exercises on closing the gender gap in political ambition. Journal of Political Science Education, 10(1), 48–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, D., & Lawless, J. (2015). A non-gendered lens? Media, voters, and female candidates in contemporary congressional elections. Perspectives on Politics, 13(1), 95–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holman, M., Merolla, J., & Zechmeister, E. (2016). Terrorist threat, male stereotypes, and candidate evaluations. Political Research Quarterly, 69(1), 134–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holman, M., Merolla, J., & Zechmeister, E. (2017). Can experience overcome stereotypes in times of terror threat? Research & Politics, 4(1), 2053168016688121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). The consequences of gender stereotypes for women candidates at different levels and types of office. Political Research Quarterly, 46(3), 503–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28(3), 280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krupnikov, Y., Piston, S., & Bauer, N. M. (2016). Saving face: Identifying voter responses to black candidates and female candidates. Political Psychology, 37(2), 253–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, J. L., & Fox, R. L. (2005). It takes a candidate: Why women don’t run for office. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, J. L., & Fox, R. L. (2010). It still takes a candidate: Why women don’t run for office. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, J., & Steigerwalt, A. (2018). Gendered vulnerability: How women work harder to stay in office. University of Michigan Press.

  • Lenz, G., & Lawson, C. (2011). Looking the part: Television leads less informed citizens to vote based on candidates’ appearance. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 574–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mansbridge, J. (1999). Should blacks represent blacks and women represent women? A contingent yes. The Journal of Politics, 61(3), 628–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayhew, D. (1974). Congress: The electoral connection. (Vol. 26). Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, R., & Williams, K. (2008). Experimentation in political science. The Oxford Handbook of Political Methodology 339–356.

  • Niven, D., & Zilber, J. (2001). Do women and men in congress cultivate different images? Evidence from congressional web sites. Political Communication, 18(4), 395–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, P. (2013). Hillary Clinton exits with 69% approval rating. The Wall Street Journal.

  • Oliver, S., & Conroy, M. C. (2016). Tough enough for the job? How masculinity predicts recruitment of city council members. American Politics Research 1532673X17729719.

  • Pillai, R., & Williams, E. A. (1998). Does leadership matter in the political arena? Voter perceptions of candidates’ transformational and charismatic leadership and the 1996 US president. The Leadership Quarterly, 9(3), 397–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preece, J., & Stoddard, O. (2015). Why women don’t run: Experimental evidence on gender differences in political competition aversion. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 117, 296–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Z., & Miles, H. (2001). Subtyping and subgrouping: Processes for the prevention and promotion of stereotype change. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(1), 52–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenwasser, S. M., & Dean, N. (1989). Gender role and political office: Effects of perceived masculinity/femininity of candidate and political office. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 13(1), 77–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanbonmatsu, K. (2006). Do parties know that “women win”? Party leader beliefs about women’s electoral chances. Politics & Gender, 2(4), 431–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanbonmatsu, K., & Dolan, K. (2009). Do gender stereotypes transcend party? Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 485–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, M., & Bos, A. (2014). Measuring stereotypes of female politicians. Political Psychology, 35(2), 245–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12040

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, M., Holman, M., Diekman, A., & McAndrew, T. (2016). Power, conflict, and community: How gendered views of political power influence women’s political ambition. Political Psychology, 37(4), 515–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stalsburg, B. (2010). Voting for mom: The political consequences of being a parent for male and female candidates. Politics & Gender, 6(3), 373–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streb, M. J., Burrell, B., Frederick, B., & Genovese, M. A. (2008). Social desirability effects and support for a female American president. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(1), 76–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweet-Cushman, J. (2016). Gender, risk assessment, and political ambition. Politics & Life Sciences, 35(2), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweet-Cushman, J. (2020). Gendered legislative effectiveness in state legislatures: the case of Pennsylvania. In N. M. Bauer (Ed.), Politicking while female.LSU Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volden, C., Wiseman, A. E., & Wittmer, D. E. (2013). When are women more effective lawmakers than men? American Journal of Political Science, 57(2), 326–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wayne, S., & Edwards, G. (2010). Presidential leadership: Politics and policy making. Wadsworth/Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Earlier versions of this manuscript were presented at the 2020 Southern Political Science Association meeting and the 2019 International Society of Political Psychology meeting. I am grateful for funding from the American Political Science Association Small Grants Fund and the Chatham University Research Sabbatical Committee and appreciate comments on earlier drafts from Nichole Bauer, Erin Cassese, Kathy Dolan, Heather Ondercin, and Mirya Holman. Special thanks to my undergraduate research assistants Sivan Nizan, Hanna Theile, and Miranda Bruno who helped with coding. Replication materials are available at: https://www.jenniesweet-cushman.com/research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennie Sweet-Cushman.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 10316 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sweet-Cushman, J. Legislative vs. Executive Political Offices: How Gender Stereotypes Can Disadvantage Women in Either Office. Polit Behav 44, 411–434 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09721-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09721-x

Navigation