Skip to main content
Log in

The Feedback Effects of Controversial Police Use of Force

Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Controversial cases of police use of force against minority civilians have become a ubiquitous feature of news headlines, and videos of these interactions between citizens and government actors have placed them in the public sphere. In this paper, we examine the feedback effects of these publicized incidents. Using a unique survey-experiment implemented in 2019, we demonstrate that controversial police use of force against minority civilians prompts strong emotional reactions, increases support for body-cameras, changes beliefs about excessive force, and alters attitudes toward law enforcement. Notably, our design allows us to examine the effects of both text-based news stories and videos pulled from two real-world use of force cases, one lethal and one non-lethal. This study has important implications for public opinion, feedback effects, and perceptions of law enforcement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Throughout this paper, we refer to “controversial” instances of police use of force. While we have personal views of specific encounters, we do not label particular situations as “excessive” or “justifiable.” For decades, researchers have discussed what constitutes legitimate use of force (e.g. Bittner 1970), but the criteria have also changed over time. Because police departments, courts, and the public have different beliefs about what constitutes excessive force, we refer to them here as “controversial.”

  2. The informed consent stated, “It is possible that reading or viewing information about these events may cause some level of emotional or psychological discomfort. Please be aware that you are free to end your participation in the survey and exit at any time.” The screen before the video stated, “The video shows a controversial altercation between a police officer and a civilian, and this may be uncomfortable for some people to watch. If you do not feel that you can watch the video in its entirety, just continue forward in the survey.” The caption under the video stated “Please click the play button to watch the video. The video contains graphic content.”

  3. Dynata participants rate their overall satisfaction with surveys they complete. Their average satisfaction rating across all recent studies is 75 (out of 100); our study received a satisfaction rating of 86.

  4. For statistics about police contacts, use of force, and differences by race in the U.S., see Davis et al. (2018).

  5. This design does not account for two other important situations: non-lethal/high salience and lethal/low salience. Our design, instead, focuses on the conditions that provide the broadest range of potential effects. The low-salience non-lethal situations are abundant in the real world, and there are reasons to question how impactful they might be (relative to the other options). They also help us avoid issues associated with pretreatment exposure that might be tied to the high-salience case. That we largely find the same pattern of results for both situations (at the opposing ends of the spectrum) leads us to believe that the conditions we did not include would likely have similar effects. With respect to pretreatment, toward the end of the survey, we asked, “How familiar are you with circumstances surrounding the shooting of Philando Castile?” If we restrict our analysis to the control group (which is uncontaminated by treatments), 59.44% of respondents said “not familiar at all.”

  6. The original non-lethal video was approximately nine minutes, but included the officer exiting his vehicle and walking to the civilian’s car and a period after the main incident, in which nothing of note is visible. We cut just over two minutes to focus the treatment video on the time from the beginning of the interaction until the scene was secured.

  7. The sample (collected in June 2019) was 57% female, 66.61% non-Hispanic White, 12.29% non-Hispanic black, and 38.65% liberal. The median education level was Associate’s degree, and median income was 50–59 k. The sample was matched based on age, gender, ethnicity, region, and partisanship.

  8. The data and replication code for all analyses is available at the following link: https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/Q7SFJW.

  9. Treatment effects may be mediated through any number of variables (e.g., emotional responses). We did not establish mediation hypotheses and did not design our experiment to test mediation. Tests for mediation using our design require many assumptions that are often untestable and unjustified.

  10. The wording of these questions allows for consistency across conditions, but we recognize that they do not tell us precisely at whom or what the emotions are directed.

  11. In the model with controls, the effect of the Non-Lethal Video for confidence in appropriate legal consequences is not significant.

References

  • Aaroe, L. (2011). Investigating frame strength: The case of episodic and thematic frames. Political Communication, 28(2), 207–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner, F. R., DeBoef, S. L., & Boydstun, A. E. (2008). The decline of the death penalty and the discovery of innocence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bittner, E. (1970). The functions of the police in modern society. New York: National Institute of Mental Health.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T., & Palm, R. (2019). Motivated reasoning and political decision making. In D. P. Redlawsk (Ed.), Oxford encyclopedia of political decision making. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., MacKenzie, S. A., & Simmons, D. J. (2019). Police violence and public perceptions. Journal of Politics, 81(3), 1101–1110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braga, A. A. (2016). Better policing can improve legitimacy and reduce mass incarceration. Harvard Law Review Forum, 129, 233–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, B., & Benedict, W. R. (2002). Perceptions of the police: Past findings, methodological issues, conceptual issues, and policy implications. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management, 25(3), 543–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burstein, P. (2003). The impact of public opinion on public policy: A review and an agenda. Political Research Quarterly, 56(1), 29–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busby, E., Flynn, D. J., & Druckman, J. N. (2018). Studying framing effects on political preferences: Existing research and lingering questions. In P. D’Angelo (Ed.), Doing news framing analysis II. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E., Gunderson, A., Jackson, K., Zachary, P., Clark, T. S., Glynn, A. N., et al. (2019). Do officer-involved shootings reduce citizen contact with government? Journal of Politics, 81(3), 1111–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Covey, R. (2013). Police misconduct as a cause of wrongful convictions. Washington University Law Review, 90, 1133–1189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crigler, A. N., Just, M., & Neuman, W. R. (1994). Interpreting visual versus audio messages in television news. Journal of Communication, 44(4), 132–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Culhane, S. E., Boman, J. H., IV, & Schweitzer, K. (2016). Public perceptions of the justifiability of police shootings: The role of body cameras in a pre- and post-Ferguson experiment. Police Quarterly, 19(3), 251–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, E., Whyde, A., & Langton, L. (2018). Contacts between police and the public, 2015. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice Office of Justice Programs: Bureau of Justice Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: Pretreatment and its effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekins, E. E. (2016). Policing in America: Understanding public attitudes toward the police. Results from a National Survey. https://ssrn.com/abstract=2919449.

  • Engel, R. S., McManus, H. D., & Isaza, G. T. (2020). Moving beyond “best practice”: Experiences in police reform and a call for evidence to reduce officer-involved shootings. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 687(1), 146–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epp, C. R., Maynard-Moody, S., & Haider-Markel, D. (2014). Pulled over: How police stops define race and citizenship. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gallup. (2019). Confidence in institutions. Retrieved June 30, 2020, from https://news.gallup.com/poll/1597/confidence-institutions.aspx.

  • Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobo, J., & Francis, E. (2016). Cops may have thought Philando Castile was a robbery suspect, noting ‘wide-set nose,’ dispatch audio indicates. ABC News, July 11. Retrieved August 26, 2019, from https://abcnews.go.com/US/cops-thought-philando-castile-robbery-suspect-dispatch-audio/story?id=40439957.

  • Khan, A. (2019). Getting killed by police is a leading cause of death for young black men in America. Los Angeles Times, August 16. Retrieved August 27, 2019, from https://www.latimes.com/science/story/2019-08-15/police-shootings-are-a-leading-cause-of-death-for-black-men.

  • Laniyonu, A. (2018). Police, politics and participation: The effect of police exposure on political participation in the United Kingdom. British Journal of Criminology, 58(5), 1232–1253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laniyonu, A. (2019). The political consequences of policing: Evidence from New York City. Political Behavior, 41, 527–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, A. E., & Harney, J. (2019). Feedback effects and the criminal justice bureaucracy: Officer attitudes and the future of correctional reform. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 685(1), 227–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, A. E., & McCabe, K. T. (2017). Personal experience and public opinion: A theory and test of conditional policy feedback. Journal of Politics, 79(2), 624–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mettler, S., & SoRelle, M. (2018). Policy feedback theory. In C. M. Weible & P. A. Sabatier (Eds.), Theories of the policy process (4th ed., pp. 103–134). New York: Westview Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. M., Nyhan, B., & Torres, M. (2018). How conditioning on posttreatment variables can ruin your experiment and what to do about it. American Journal of Political Science, 62(3), 760–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, K. (2018). Body camera video shows use of force by Topeka officer that led to lawsuit. Topeka Capital Journal, September 25. Retrieved October 4, 2019, from https://www.cjonline.com/news/20180925/body-camera-video-shows-use-of-force-by-topeka-officer-that-led-to-lawsuit.

  • Mullinix, K. J., & Norris, R. J. (2019). Pulled-over rates, causal attributions, and trust in police. Political Research Quarterly, 72(2), 420–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C., & Reeves, B. (2005). The new videomalaise: Effects of televised incivility on political trust. American Political Science Review, 99(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Justice. (2013). Race, trust, and police legitimacy. Retrieved October 6, 2019, from https://www.nij.gov/topics/law-enforcement/legitimacy/Pages/welcome.aspx.

  • Norris, R. J., & Mullinix, K. J. (2019). Framing innocence: An experimental test of wrongful convictions and public opinion. Journal of Experimental Criminology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-019-09360-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peffley, M., & Hurwitz, J. (2010). Justice in America: The separate realities of blacks and whites. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, T. E., Boomgaarden, H. G., De Swert, K., & de Vreese, C. H. (2015). A clearer picture: The contribution of visuals and text to framing effects. Journal of Communication, 65(6), 997–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schafer, J. A., Huebner, B. M., & Bynum, T. S. (2003). Citizen perceptions of police services: Race, neighborhood context, and community policing. Police Quarterly, 6(4), 440–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schattschneider, E. E. (1935). Politics, pressure, and the tariff. New York, NY: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, E. A., Jones, M. D., McBeth, M. K., & Radaelli, C. M. (2018). The narrative policy framework. In C. M. Weible & P. A. Sabatier (Eds.), Theories of the policy process (4th ed.). New York: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, L. W. (2020). Evidence-based policing and fatal police shootings: Promise, problems, and prospects. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 687(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tal-Or, N., Boninger, D. S., Poran, A., & Gleicher, F. (2004). Counterfactual thinking as a mechanism in narrative persuasion. Human Communication Research, 30(3), 301–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Testa, P., & Dietrich, B. J. (2017). Seeing is believing: How video of police action affects criminal justice beliefs. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved October 4, 2019, from https://paultesta.org/pdf/TestaDietrich_APSA2017.pdf.

  • Turner, B. L., Caruso, E. M., Dilich, M. A., & Roese, N. J. (2019). Body camera footage leads to lower judgments of intent than dash camera footage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 116(4), 1201–1206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, V. M., & Geller, A. (2019). De-policing America’s youth: Disrupting criminal justice policy feedbacks that distort power and derail prospects. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 685(1), 190–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weitzer, R., & Tuch, S. A. (2005). Determinants of public satisfaction with the police. Police Quarterly, 8(3), 279–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, M. D., & Fradella, H. F. (2016). Stop and frisk: The use and abuse of a controversial policing tactic. New York: NYU Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, J., & Toronjo, H. (2019). Translating police research into policy: Some implications of the National Academies report on proactive policing for policymakers and researchers. Police Practice and Research. https://doi.org/10.1080/15614263.2019.1657631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y., Sun, I. Y., & Triplett, R. A. (2009). Race, class or neighborhood context: Which matters more in measuring satisfaction with police? Justice Quarterly, 26(1), 125–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. (2014). Race/ethnicity and perceptions of the police: A comparison of White, Black, Asian and Hispanic Americans. Policing and Society, 24(2), 135–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimring, F. E. (2020). Police killings as a problem of governance. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 687(1), 114–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank James Willis, Mark Peffley, Donald Haider-Markel, and Judd Thornton for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. This study was funded by a Faculty Research and Development Award from the College of Humanities and Social Sciences at George Mason University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Toby Bolsen.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 27831 kb)

Supplementary file2 (DOCX 135 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mullinix, K.J., Bolsen, T. & Norris, R.J. The Feedback Effects of Controversial Police Use of Force. Polit Behav 43, 881–898 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09646-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09646-x

Keywords

Navigation