Skip to main content
Log in

Risk Attitudes and Independence Vote Choice

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this article, we examine the impact of risk attitudes on vote choice in the context of a salient referendum with high levels of uncertainty about the consequences of the ballot proposal. Using data from a pre- and post-referendum panel survey conducted in the context of the 2014 independence referendum in Scotland, and a specific battery to measure attitudes to risk, we determine how these attitudes operate in such political contexts. We reach two main conclusions. First, risk attitudes have a direct effect on vote choice, even after controlling for alternative explanations of vote choice such as party identification and leaders’ evaluations. In the aggregate, the effect of risk attitudes on the vote choice contributes to the status quo bias found in referendums. Second, we find that information moderates the effect of risk attitudes on vote choice. Voters who are politically knowledgeable have a greater capacity to predict the consequences of political outcomes and, therefore, they are less affected by their risk attitudes when making their ballot choices.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Source http://whatscotlandthinks.org. The figure gathers the answers to the exact question ‘Should Scotland be an independent country?’ on any representative survey of Scotland’s population conducted by polling companies that belong to the British Polling Council (n = 99). The trend lines are estimated using local polynomial regression, locally estimated scatter plot smoothing (LOESS)

Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The massive turnout levels achieved in the independence referendums of Quebec and Scotland indicate the saliency of the choice. In Quebec, turnout was 85.6% in 1980 and 93.5% in 1995. In Scotland 2014, turnout was 84.6%. Both the 1995 referendum and the 2014 established turnout records in Quebec and Scotland respectively.

  2. Although the no-vote usually implies no changes, this is not necessarily the case. In order to take into account the possibility that a vote against the proposal does not lead to a continuation of the pre-existing status quo, the literature distinguishes between the ballot proposal and the reversion point—the situation that prevails in the event of a no-vote—as the main alternatives of a binary-choice referendum (Hobolt 2009, pp. 45–46). In the context of independence referendums, a vote against secession does not necessarily mean that changes will not occur, but it is difficult to think of a context in which the uncertainties related to a no-vote can be compared to the unknowns associated to a yes-vote.

  3. The clear road to the referendum enhanced the legitimacy of the vote further. The referendum was triggered by the Scottish National Party (SNP) victory in the May 2011 elections to the Scottish Parliament. The SNP, which included the promise of holding an independence referendum in its manifesto, was the first party ever to achieve a majority in the Scottish Parliament, elected through an additional member system. This majority was interpreted by both the Scottish and the UK government as a mandate to hold an independence referendum.

  4. The polls gathered in Fig. 1 signal the peak support for independence 1 week before the vote, a support that slightly dropped in the final days of the campaign. Actually, 5 out of the 6 polling companies that measured surveys in the last weeks of the campaign show a drop in the support to independence between the penultimate and the last survey; the other polling company showed no change between the last two surveys.

  5. In the Scotland independence referendum, the electoral franchise was extended to include 16- and 17-year-olds.

  6. The online fieldwork was conducted by ICM. The pre-referendum data were collected from 9 June 2014 to 30 June 2014, whereas the post-referendum data were collected from 2 December 2014 to 23 December 2014. The data can be downloaded from http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-8247-1.

  7. Collinearity tests reveal no multicollinearity issues. None of the independent variables specified in Tables 1 and 2 show a variance inflation factor higher than 1.9.

  8. See Table 3 in the “Appendix” section for details on the measurement of independent variables and descriptive statistics.

  9. This is an important point because one of the main criticisms of Clarke et al. (2004) of the model produced by Nadeau et al. (1999) in the context of the 1995 Quebec referendum was that key variables political variables such as party identification and feelings about party leaders were absent in their model specification. They also criticize the fact that Nadeau et al. (1999) find interactive but not main effects of risk attitudes, noting that ‘it is difficult to argue that general orientations to risk impact on referendum choices but that there is no main effect’ (Clarke et al. 2004, p. 349).

  10. Holding variables at their means indicates that we use the sample mean in the case of cardinal-level variables and sample proportions for the other types of variables to calculate our probabilities.

  11. Three of the items involved showing pictures of different political leaders to our respondents and giving them four options about the office that each leader occupies. Only one option was correct. The political figures were Iain Duncan Smith (then Secretary of State for Work and Pensions); Hermann Van Rompuy (then President of the European Council); and Ruth Davidson (leader of the Scottish Conservative Party). Two additional questions also offered voters four options: ‘In which year did New Labour under Tony Blair first form a government?’ (correct answer ‘1997’), and ‘Which of the following positions does the Conservative Party hold in Westminster?’ (correct answer was ‘The Conservative party heads a coalition government’).

  12. Figure 4 in the “Appendix” section displays the average marginal effects (Ai and Norton 2003; Clarke et al. 2015; Karaca-Mandic et al. 2012).

References

  • Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez, R. M. (1997). Information and elections. Ann Arbor: Michigan University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez, R. M., & Brehm, J. (2002). Hard choices, easy answers: Values, information, and American public opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1986). Issue voting under uncertainty: An empirical test. American Journal of Political Science, 30(4), 709–728.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bélanger, É., & Perrella, A. M. L. (2008). Facteurs d’appui à la souveraineté du Québec chez les jeunes: Une comparaison entre francophones, anglophones et allophones. Politique et Sociétés, 27(3), 13–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, M. M., Munger, M. C., & Potthoff, R. F. (2000). The Downsian model predicts divergence. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 12(2), 228–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berinski, A. J., & Lewis, J. B. (2007). An estimate of risk aversion in the U.S. electorate. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 2, 139–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blais, A., & Nadeau, R. (1992). To be or not to be sovereignist: Quebeckers’ perennial dilemma. Canadian Public Policy/Analyse de Politiques, 18(1), 89–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, R. (2000). Squaring the circles: Demonstrating and explaining the political “non-alignment” of Scottish national identity. Scottish Affairs, 32(Summer), 15–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, S., & Donovan, T. (1998). Demanding choices. Ann Arbor: Michigan University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carducci, B. J., & Wong, A. S. (1998). Type A and risk taking in everyday money matters. Journal of Business and Psychology, 12(3), 355–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2002). Undivided loyalties is national identity an obstacle to European integration? European Union Politics, 3(4), 387–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Wallace, B. (2009). Genetic variation in preferences for giving and risk taking. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124(2), 809–842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christin, T., Hug, S., & Sciarini, P. (2002). Interests and information in referendum voting: An analysis of Swiss voters. European Journal of Political Research, 41(6), 759–776.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. D., Elliott, E., & Stewart, M. C. (2015). Heuristics, heterogeneity and green choices voting on California’s proposition 23. Political Science Research and Methods, 5(4), 755–774.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. D., & Kornberg, A. (1994). The politics and economics of constitutional choice: Voting in Canada’s 1992 national referendum. The Journal of Politics, 56(04), 940–962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. D., & Kornberg, A. (1996). Choosing Canada? The 1995 Quebec sovereignty referendum. Political Science & Politics, 29(04), 676–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. D., Kornberg, A., & Stewart, M. C. (2004). Referendum voting as political choice: The case of Quebec. British Journal of Political Science, 34(02), 345–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The Nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: The Free Press of Glencoe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1975). Public opinion and voting behaviour. In F. I. Greenstein & N. W. Polsby (Eds.), Handbook of political science nongovernmental politics (Vol. 4, pp. 75–169). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (2000). Assessing the capacity of mass electorates. Annual Review of Political Science, 3, 331–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlbäck, O. (1990). Personality and risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(12), 1235–1242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlbäck, O. (1991). Saving and risk taking. Journal of Economic Psychology, 12(3), 479–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, R. J. (1999). Political support in advanced democracies. In P. Norris (Ed.), Critical citizens: Global support for democratic government. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, R. J. (2004). Democratic challenges, democratic choices: The erosion of political support in advanced industrial democracies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vreese, C. H. (Ed.). (2007). The Dynamics of referendum campaigns: An international perspective. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vreese, C. H., & Semetko, H. A. (2004). Political campaigning in referendums: Framing the referendum issue. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9(3), 522–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckles, D. L., Kam, C. D., Maestas, C. L., & Schaffner, B. F. (2013). Risk attitudes and the incumbency advantage. Political Behavior, 36(4), 731–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckles, D. L., & Schaffner, B. F. (2011). Risk tolerance and support for potential military interventions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 533–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, S., & Maestas, C. (2010). Risk orientation, risk exposure, and policy opinions: The Case of free trade. Political Psychology, 31(5), 657–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eidelman, S., & Crandall, C. S. (2009). Psychological Advantage for the status quo. In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eidelman, S., & Crandall, C. S. (2012). Bias in favor of the status quo. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(3), 270–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enelow, J., & Hinich, M. J. (1981). A new approach to voter uncertainty in the Downsian spatial model. American Journal of Political Science, 25(3), 483–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filbeck, G., Hatfield, P., & Horvath, P. (2005). Risk aversion and personality type. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 6(4), 170–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, M. N., van der Eijk, C., & Marsh, M. (1995). Referendum outcomes and trust in government: Public support for Europe in the wake of Maastricht. West European Politics, 18(3), 101–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halek, M., & Eisenhauer, J. G. (2001). Demography of risk aversion. The Journal of Risk and Insurance, 68(1), 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, A. (2014). The myth of meritocratic Scotland: political cultures in the UK. In P. Cowley & R. Ford (Eds.), Sex, lies and the ballot box: 50 things you need to know about British elections (pp. 103–107). London: Biteback.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobolt, S. B. (2009). Europe in question. Referendums on European Integration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hug, S., & Sciarini, P. (2000). Referendums on European integration do institutions matter in the voter’s decision? Comparative Political Studies, 33(1), 3–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, R. (2016). It wasn’t “The Vow” wot won it: the Scottish independence referendum. In P. Cowley & R. Ford (Eds.), More sex, lies and the ballot box: Another 50 things you need to know about elections (pp. 185–189). London: Biteback.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, R., Blais, A., Gidengil, E., & Nevitte, N. (1992). Challenge of direct democracy: The 1992 Canadian referendum. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D. (2012). Risk attitudes and political participation. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 817–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D., & Simas, E. N. (2010). Risk orientations and policy frames. The Journal of Politics, 72(02), 381–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D., & Simas, E. N. (2012). Risk attitudes, candidate characteristics, and vote choice. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(4), 747–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karaca-Mandic, P., Norton, E. C., & Dowd, B. (2012). Interaction terms in nonlinear models. Health Services Research, 47(1 Pt 1), 255–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, A. C., Jimenez, M. C., & Jost, J. T. (2002). Sour grapes, sweet lemons, and the anticipatory rationalization of the status quo. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 1300–1312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, M. (Ed.). (2017). Debating Scotland: Issues of independence and union in the 2014 referendum. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kowert, P. A., & Hermann, M. G. (1997). Who takes risks? Daring and caution in foreign policy making. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 41(5), 611–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDuc, L. (2002a). Referendums and elections. How do campaigns differ? In D. M. Farrell & R. Schmitt-Beck (Eds.), Do political campaigns matter? Campaign effects in elections and referendums (pp. 145–162). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDuc, L. (2002b). Opinion change and voting behaviour in referendums. European Journal of Political Research, 41(6), 711–732.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDuc, L. (2003). The politics of direct democracy: Referendums in global perspective. Peterborough: Broadview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 75–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A. (1994). Shortcuts versus encyclopedias: Information and voting behavior in California insurance reform elections. The American Political Science Review, 88(1), 63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCrimmon, K. R., Wehrung, D. A., & Stanbury, W. T. (1986). Taking risks: The management of uncertainty. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrone, D., & Paterson, L. (2002). The conundrum of Scottish independence. Scottish Affairs, 40(1), 54–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLean, I., Lodge, G., & Gallagher, J. (2013). Scotland’s choices: The Referendum and what happens afterwards. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meertens, R. M., & Lion, R. (2008). Measuring an individual’s tendency to take risks: The risk propensity scale. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(6), 1506–1520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgenstern, S., & Zechmeister, E. (2001). Better the devil you know than the saint you don’t? Risk propensity and vote choice in Mexico. The Journal of Politics, 63(1), 93–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadeau, R., & Fleury, C. J. (1995). Gains linguistiques anticipés et appui à la souveraineté du Québec. Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue Canadienne de Science Politique, 28(01), 35–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadeau, R., Martin, P., & Blais, A. (1999). Attitude towards risk-taking and individual choice in the Quebec referendum on sovereignty. British Journal of Political Science, 29(03), 523–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, N., Soane, E., Fenton-O’Creevy, M., & Willman, P. (2005). Personality and domain-specific risk taking. Journal of Risk Research, 8(2), 157–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quattrone, G. A., & Tversky, A. (1988). Contrasting rational and psychological analyses of political choice. American Political Science Review, 82(3), 719–736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renwick, A. (2014). Don’t trust your poll lead: How public opinion changes during referendum campaigns. In P. Cowley & R. Ford (Eds.), Sex, lies and the ballot box: 50 things you need to know about British elections (pp. 79–84). London: Biteback.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renwick, A. (2017). Referendums. In K. Arzheimer, J. Evans, & M. S. Lewis-Beck (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of electoral behaviour (pp. 433–458). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley, W. B., Jr., & Chow, K. V. (1992). Asset allocation and individual risk aversion. Financial Analysts Journal, 48(6), 32–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roiser, J. P., de Martino, B., Tan, G. C. Y., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., Wood, N. W., et al. (2009). A genetically mediated bias in decision making driven by failure of amygdala control. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(18), 5985–5991.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, A. B., Tsai, J. S., & Downs, S. M. (2003). Variations in risk attitude across race, gender, and education. Medical Decision Making, 23(6), 511–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, G., & Weitsman, P. A. (1996). The punishment trap integration referendums as popularity contests. Comparative Political Studies, 28(4), 582–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scottish Government. (2013). Scotland’s future. Your Guide to an independent Scotland. Edinburgh: Scottish Government.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepsle, K. A. (1972). The strategy of ambiguity: Uncertainty and electoral competition. American Political Science Review, 66(2), 555–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steenbergen, M. R., & Siczek, T. (2017). Better the devil you know? Risk-taking, globalization and populism in Great Britain. European Union Politics, 18(1), 119–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sung, J., & Hanna, S. (1996). Factors related to risk tolerance. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning, 7, 11–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomz, M., & Van Houweling, R. P. (2009). The electoral implications of candidate ambiguity. The American Political Science Review, 103(1), 83–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, E., & Martin, J. (1969). Generality of willingness to take risks. Psychological Reports, 24(2), 499–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiteley, P., Clarke, H. D., Sanders, D., & Stewart, M. C. (2012). Britain says NO: Voting in the AV ballot referendum. Parliamentary Affairs, 65(2), 301–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaleskiewicz, T. (2001). Beyond risk seeking and risk aversion: Personality and the dual nature of economic risk taking. European Journal of Personality, 15(S1), S105–S122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. R. (1992). The Nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Abingdon: Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Common bisocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 999–1029.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was funded by FEDER/Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades - Agencia Estatal de Investigación (Spain) (Grant No. PGC2018-096081-A-I00) and by the Economic and Social Research Council (UK) (Grant No. ES/L003325/1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Liñeira.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 16 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 3 and Fig. 4.

Table 3 Descriptive statistics
Fig. 4
figure 4

Average marginal effects of risk tolerance on voting yes

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Liñeira, R., Henderson, A. Risk Attitudes and Independence Vote Choice. Polit Behav 43, 541–560 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09560-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09560-x

Keywords

Navigation