Pigeonholing Partisans: Stereotypes of Party Supporters and Partisan Polarization

  • Jacob E. Rothschild
  • Adam J. Howat
  • Richard M. Shafranek
  • Ethan C. Busby
Original Paper

Abstract

What comes to mind when people think about rank-and-file party supporters? What stereotypes do people hold regarding ordinary partisans, and are these views politically consequential? We utilize open-ended survey items and structural topic modeling to document stereotypes about rank-and-file Democrats and Republicans. Many subjects report stereotypes consistent with the parties’ actual composition, but individual differences in political knowledge, interest, and partisan affiliation predict their specific content. Respondents varied in their tendency to characterize partisans in terms of group memberships, issue preferences, or individual traits, lending support to both ideological and identity-based conceptions of partisanship. Most importantly, we show that partisan stereotype content is politically significant: individuals who think of partisans in a predominantly trait-based manner—that is, in a way consistent with partisanship as a social identity—display dramatically higher levels of both affective and ideological polarization.

Keywords

Partisanship Political parties Partisan polarization Social identity Stereotypes 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Jamie Druckman, Doug Ahler, participants in the Druckman political science research lab, participants in the Thursday group at Brigham Young University, three anonymous reviewers, and discussants at MPSA and WPSA for insightful feedback and suggestions. We also thank Brandon Stewart and Matthew Lacombe for their helpful methodological advice. All errors are our own. Financial support for this research came from the Political Science Department at Northwestern University. This research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University. The authors contributed equally to this work. Data and replication code for the analyses presented in this paper can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U23L09.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

11109_2018_9457_MOESM1_ESM.docx (57 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 56 kb)

References

  1. Abramowitz, A. I., & Saunders, K. L. (2006). Exploring the bases of partisanship in the American electorate: Social identity vs. ideology. Political Research Quarterly, 59(2), 175–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abramowitz, A. I., & Saunders, K. L. (2008). Is polarization a myth? Journal of Politics, 70(2), 542–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2016). Democracy for realists: Why elections do not produce responsive government. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ahler, D., & Sood, G. (Forthcoming). The parties in our heads: Misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. Journal of Politics. Google Scholar
  5. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  6. Ashmore, R. D., & Del Boca, F. K. (1981). Conceptual approaches to stereotypes and stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  7. Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 228–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bauer, P. C., Barberá, P., Ackermann, K., & Venetz, A. (2017). Is the left-right scale a valid measure of ideology? Individual-level variation in associations with “left” and “right” and left-right self-placement. Political Behavior, 39(3), 553–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baumer, D. C., & Gold, H. J. (1995). Party images and the American electorate. American Politics Research, 23(1), 33–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baumer, D. C., & Gold, H. J. (2007). Party images and partisan resurgence. The Social Science Journal, 44(3), 465–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  12. Biernat, M. (2003). Toward a broader view of social stereotyping. American Psychologist, 58(12), 1019–1027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bordalo, P., Coffman, K., Gennaioli, N., & Shleifer, A. (2016). Stereotypes. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 141(4), 1753–1794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Chambers, J. R., Baron, R. S., & Inman, M. L. (2006). Misperceptions in intergroup conflict: Disagreeing about what we disagree about. Psychological Science, 17(1), 38–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1139–1150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Druckman, J. N., & Kam, C. D. (2011). Students as experimental participants: A defense of the ‘narrow data base’. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science (pp. 41–56). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1989). Gender stereotypes and attitudes toward women and men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(4), 545–558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Glick, P., Diebold, J., Bailey-Werner, B., & Zhu, L. (1997). The two faces of Adam: Ambivalent sexism and polarized attitudes toward women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(12), 1323–1334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goggin, S. N., & Theodoridis, A. G. (2017). Disputed ownership: Parties, issues, and traits in the minds of voters. Political Behavior, 39(3), 675–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., & Haidt, J. (2012). The moral stereotypes of liberals and conservatives: exaggeration of differences across the political spectrum. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e50092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Green, D., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Greene, S. (1999). Understanding party identification: A social identity approach. Political Psychology, 20(2), 393–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Greene, S. (2004). Social identity theory and party identification. Social Science Quarterly, 85(1), 136–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Perceiving persons and groups. Psychological Review, 103(2), 336–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hetherington, M. J. (2001). Resurgent mass partisanship: The role of elite polarization. American Political Science Review, 95(3), 619–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hogg, M. A. (1992). The social psychology of group cohesiveness: From attraction to social identity. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relationships and group processes. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  31. Huddy, L., Mason, L., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Expressive partisanship: Campaign involvement, political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science Review, 109(1), 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Iyengar, S. (1996). Framing responsibility for political issues. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 546(1), 59–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Josefson, J. (2000). An exploration of the stability of partisan stereotypes in the United States. Party Politics, 6(3), 285–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Katz, D., & Braly, K. W. (1933). Racial stereotypes of 100 college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28(3), 280–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Key, V. O. (1964). Parties, politics, and pressure groups (5th ed.). New York: Crowell.Google Scholar
  38. Kinder, D. R., & Kalmoe, N. (2017). Neither liberal nor conservative: Ideological innocence in the American public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  39. Levay, K. E., Freese, J., & Druckman, J. N. (2016). The demographic and political composition of mechanical turk samples. SAGE Open, 6(1), 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., Hong, Y.-y., Chiu, C.-y., & Dweck, C. S. (2001). Static versus dynamic theories and the perception of groups: Different routes to different destinations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(2), 156–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1421–1436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and Company.Google Scholar
  43. Lodge, M., & Hamill, R. (1986). A partisan schema for political information processing. American Political Science Review, 80(2), 505–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Macrae, C. Neil, & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Madon, S., Guyll, M., Aboufadel, K., Montiel, E., Smith, A., Palumbo, P., et al. (2001). Ethnic and national stereotypes: The Princeton trilogy revisited and revised. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(8), 996–1010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mason, L. (2013). The rise of uncivil agreement: Issue versus behavioral polarization in the American electorate. American Behavioral Scientist, 57(1), 140–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mason, L. (2015). ‘I disrespectfully agree’: The differential effects of partisan sorting on social and issue polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 128–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mason, L. (2016). A cross-cutting calm: How social sorting drives affective polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 80(S1), 351–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mullinix, K. J., Leeper, T. J., Druckman, J. N., & Freese, J. (2015). The generalizability of survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 2(2), 109–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92(3), 289–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Niemi, R. G., & Jennings, M. K. (1991). Issues and inheritance in the formation of party identification. American Journal of Political Science, 35(4), 970–988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rahn, W. M. (1993). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rahn, W. M., & Cramer, K. J. (1996). Activation and application of political party stereotypes: The role of television. Political Communication, 13(2), 195–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., & Airoldi, E. M. (2016). A model of text for experimentation in the social sciences. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 111(515), 988–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., & Tingley, D. (2017). stm: R package for Structural Topic Models, version 1.2.1. http://www.structuraltopicmodel.com.
  56. Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., Tingley, D., Lucas, C., Leder-Luis, J., Gadarian, S. K., et al. (2014). Structural topic models for open-ended survey responses. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 1064–1082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Scherer, A. M., Windschitl, P. D., & Graham, J. (2015). An ideological house of mirrors: Political stereotypes as exaggerations of motivated social cognition differences. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(2), 201–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sherman, J. W., Kruschke, J. K., Sherman, S. J., Percy, E. J., Petrocelli, J. V., & Conrey, F. R. (2009). Attentional processes in stereotype formation: A common model for category accentuation and illusory correlation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 305–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sniderman, P. M., & Stiglitz, E. H. (2012). The reputational premium: A theory of party identification and policy reasoning. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Stangor, C., & Lange, J. E. (1994). Mental representations of social groups: Advances in understanding stereotypes and stereotyping. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 357–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin (Ed.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Theodoridis, A.G. (2017). Me, myself, and (I), (D), or (R)? Partisanship and political cognition through the lens of implicit identity. Journal of Politics. Google Scholar
  64. Westfall, J., Van Boven, L., Chambers, J. R., & Judd, C. M. (2015). Perceiving political polarization in America: Party identity strength and attitude extremity exacerbate the perceived partisan divide. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 145–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Yzerbyt, V., Rocher, S., Schadron, G. (1997). Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective essentialistic view of group perception. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp. 20–50). Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jacob E. Rothschild
    • 1
  • Adam J. Howat
    • 1
  • Richard M. Shafranek
    • 1
  • Ethan C. Busby
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Political ScienceNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations