Abstract
Does pre-existing preference based on skin tone, facial features, and other observable characteristics, i.e., phenotypic preference, affect immigrant voters’ support for political candidates competing in their countries of origin? Do these preferences change as migrants’ tenure in their host society increases? These questions are important for ethnic and racial politics in general, and particularly for the sizable foreign-born population in the United States, which includes 11 million Mexicans. Using a unique, random sample of foreign-born Mexicans in San Diego County, we employ a voting experiment to test the impact of skin tone and phenotype on vote choice among first generation immigrants. Our design allows us to distinguish responses to different phenotypic cues by exposing respondents to European, mestizo, and indigenous looking candidates competing in a hypothetical Mexican election. Migrants showed higher support for the Indigenous candidate, and evaluated the European and Mestizo candidates as more ideologically conservative. As migrants’ time in the United States increases, the preference for indigenous features gives way to a preference for whiteness, which we interpret as evidence of first generation migrants adopting the dominant racial ideology of the United States. While ethnic distinctions have long been viewed as a key component of voting behavior, our research demonstrates that, even within a single ethnicity, racial differences may have profound impacts on the evaluation of and support for electoral candidates. This study contributes to the research on race and political behavior in comparative perspective, as well as the political consequences of migration.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
For a general approach to the use of physical features in forming opinions about others, see Ashmore and del Boca (1981), Fiske and Taylor (1984); for specific applications to evaluations of political candidates, see Terkildsen (1993), Maddox (2004), Kam (2007), McConnaughy et al. (2010), Weaver (2012), Aguilar (2015).
In what follows, we drop the term “looking” from our descriptions of candidate appearance.
A detailed description of the methodology used to build the sampling frame for this study is provided in Marcelli (2014).
To our knowledge, no research to date has examined the relationship between party labels and candidates’ phenotypic appearance in Mexico. We chose to present independent candidates in order to avoid contaminating the treatment with additional, potentially conflicting signals about party identification.
Nayarit is one of the smallest Mexican states by population, and thus has contributed only a small share to overall Mexico-U.S. migration. While we did not capture respondents’ state of origin, it is likely that almost none were natives of Nayarit.
Participants were informed, at the time of recruitment, that they could choose not to answer any survey item.
The differences between these two conditions and the control group are also significant in a regression including the same controls that appear in Table 1 (results not shown).
More than 60% of this sample had lived in the U.S. more than 20 years.
Age, Sex and Education were all set to their median values.
In regression results, not shown but included in the online data and analysis file, we also show that subjects in the control condition (those who saw no candidate image) demonstrated no statistically significant relationship between time in the U.S. and preference for the target candidate. This further reinforces our contention that the findings in support of our Hypothesis 3 are driven by subjects’ reactions to the experimental cue, and not by differences in the political views of more recent and more experienced migrants in our sample.
References
Adida, C. L., Davenport, L. D., & McClendon, G. (2016). Ethnic cueing across minorities: A survey experiment on candidate evaluation in the United States. Public Opinion Quarterly, 4(80), 815–836.
Aguilar, R. (2009). The Political Consequences of Prejudice among Mexicans and Mexican Americans. Thesis University of Michigan.
Aguilar, R. (2011) Social and Political Consequences of Stereotypes Related to Racial Phenotypes in Mexico. CIDE Working Paper, DTEP-230.
Aguilar, R. (2015). The Effect of Context on the Expression of Prejudice: Electoral Behavior in the U.S. and Mexico. CIDE.
Aguilar, R., Cunow, S., Desposato, S., & Barone, L. (2015). Ballot structure, candidate race, and vote choice in Brazil. Latin American Research Review, 50(3), 175–202.
Allport, G. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley.
Ashmore, R. D., & del Boca, F. K. (1981). Sex stereotypes and implicit personality theory. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive Processes & in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baker, A., & Greene, K. F. (2015). Positional issue voting in Latin America. In R. E. Carlin, M. M. Singer, & E.-J. Zechmeister (Eds.), The Latin American Voter: Pursuing Representation and Accountability in Challenging Contexts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Barreto, M. A. (2005). Latino immigrants at the polls: Foreign-born voter turnout in the 2002 election. Political Research Quarterly, 58(1), 79–86.
Barreto, M. A. (2007). iSí Se Puede! Latino candidates and the mobilization of Latino voters. American Political Science Review, 101(3), 425–441.
Berinsky, A. J., & Mendelberg, T. (2005). The indirect effects of discredited stereotypes in judgments of Jewish leaders. American Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 845–864.
Boas, T., & Smith, A. E. (2015). Religion and the Latin American Voter. In R. E. Carlin, M. M. Singer, & E. J. Zechmeister (Eds.), The Latin American Voter: Pursuing Representation and Accountability in Challenging Contexts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Bueno, N. S., & Dunning, T. (2017). Race, resources, and representation: Evidence from Brazilian politicians. World Politics, 69(2), 327–365.
Callghan, K., & Terkildsen, N. (2002). Understanding the role of race in candidate evaluation. Political Decision Making, Deliberation, and Participation, 6, 51–95.
Canache, D., Hayes, M., Mondak, J. J., & Seligson, M. A. (2014). Determinants of perceived skin-color discrimination in Latin America. The Journal of Politics, 76(2), 506–520.
Carlin, R. E., Singer, M. M., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2015). Conclusion. In R. E. Carlin, M. M. Singer, & E. J. Zechmeister, (eds.), The Latin American Voter: Pursuing Representation and Accountability in Challenging Contexts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Colleau, S. M., Glynn, K., Lybrand, S., Merelman, R. M., Mohan, P., & Wall, J. E. (1990). Symbolic racism in candidate evaluation: An experiment. Political Behavior, 12(4), 385–402.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5.
Domínguez, J. I., & Lawson, C. H. (2004). Mexico’s Pivotal Democratic Election: Candidates, Voters, and the Presidential Campaign of 2000. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Domínguez, J. I., Lawson, C. H., & Moreno, A. (2009). Consolidating Mexico’s Democracy: The 2006 Presidential Campaign in Comparative Perspective. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Doremus, A. (2001). Indigenism, Mestizaje, and National Identity in Mexico during the 1940s and the 1950s. Mexican Studies/Estudios Mexicanos, 17(2), 375–402.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social Cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co.
Flores, R., & Telles, E. (2012). Social stratification in Mexico: Disentangling color, ethnicity, and class. American Sociological Review, 77(3), 486–494.
Gomez, B. T., & Wilson, J. M. (2006). Cognitive heterogeneity and economic voting: A comparative analysis of four democratic electorates. American Journal of Political Science, 50(1), 127–145.
Graves, S., & Lee, J. (2000). Ethnic underpinnings of voting preference: Latinos and the 1996 US senate election in Texas. Social Science Quarterly, 81, 226–236.
Greene, K. F. (2011). Campaign persuasion and nascent partisanship in Mexico’s new democracy. American Journal of Political Science, 55(2), 398–416.
Hippel, V., William, D. S., & Vargas, P. (1997). The linguistic intergroup bias as an implicit indicator of prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5), 490–509.
Highton, B. (2004). White voters and African American candidates for congress. Political Behavior, 26(1), 1–25.
Juenke, E. G. (2014). Ignorance is bias: The effect of Latino Losers on models of Latino representation. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 593–603.
Kam, C. D. (2007). Implicit attitudes, explicit choices: When subliminal priming predicts candidate preferences. Political Behavior, 29, 343–367.
Karlins, M., Coffman, T. L., & Walters, G. (1969). On the fading of social stereotypes: Studies in three generations of college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13(1), 1.
Kinder, D. R. (1986). Presidential Character Revisited. Political Cognition 19, 474.
Klesner, J. L. (2002). Presidential and congressional elections in Mexico, July 2000. Electoral Studies, 21(1), 140–147.
Klesner, J. L. (2005). Electoral competition and the new party system in Mexico. Latin American Politics and Society, 47(2), 103–142.
Knight, A. (1990). Racism, Revolution and Indigenismo: Mexico, 1910–1940. In The Idea (Ed.), of Race in Latin America (pp. 1870–1940). Austin: University of Texas Press.
Lawson, C., Lenz, G. S., Baker, A., & Myers, M. (2010). Looking like a winner: Candidate appearance and electoral success in new democracies. World Politics, 62(4), 561–593.
Lawson, C., & McCann, J. A. (2005). Television news, Mexico’s 2000 elections and media effects in emerging democracies. British Journal of Political Science, 35(1), 1–30.
Lerman, A. E., McCabe, K. T., & Sadin, M. L. (2015). Political ideology, skin tone, and the psychology of candidate evaluations. Public Opinion Quarterly, 79(1), 53–90.
Lin, W., Green, D. P., & Coppock, A. (2017). Standard Operating Procedures for Don Green’s Lab at Columbia. https://github.com/acoppock/Green-Lab-SOP. Accessed: 27 October 2017.
Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 383–401.
Mainwaring, S., Torcal, M., & Somma, N. (2015). The Left and the Mobilization of Class Voting in Latin America. In Carlin, R. E., Singer, M. M., Zechmeister, E. J. (eds.), The Latin American Voter: Pursuing Representation and Accountability in Challenging Contexts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Manzano, S., & Sanchez, G. R. (2010). Take one for the team? Limits of shared ethnicity and candidate preferences. Political Research Quarterly, 63(3), 568–580.
Marcelli, E. A. (2014). Community-based migrant household probability sampling. In Schenker, M., Castaneda, X., & Rodrizuez-Lainz, A. (eds.), Migration and Health Research Methodologies: A Handbook for the Study of Migrant Populations. Ann Arbor: University of California Press.
McConnaughy, C., White, D. L., & Casellas, J. (2010). A Latino on the ballot: Explaining coethnic voting among Latinos and the response of white Americans. Journal of Politics, 72(4), 1199–1211.
Mendelberg, T. (2001). The Race Card: Campaign Strategy, Implicit Messages, and the Norm of Equality. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Moreno, A. (2009). La decisión electoral: votantes, partidos y democracia. Miguel Ángel Porrúa: México.
Moskowitz, D., & Stroh, P. (1994). Psychological sources of electoral racism. Political Psychology (pp. 307–329).
Pérez, E. O. (2016). Unspoken Politics: Implicit Attitudes and Political Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Perlmann, J., & Waters, M. C. (2002). The new race question: How the census counts multiracial individuals. New Yorl: Russell Sage Foundation.
Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Ramakrishnan, S. K., & Espenshade, T. J. (2001). Immigrant incorporation and political participation in the United States. International Migration Review, 35(3), 870–909.
Reeves, K. (1997). Voting Hopes or Fears?: White Voters, Black Candidates, and Racial Politics in America. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sanbonmatsu, K. (2002). Gender stereotypes and vote choice. American Journal of Political Science 46, 20–34.
Sears, D. O, Citrin, J., & Kosterman, R. (1987). Jesse Jackson and the southern white electorate in 1984. In Moreland, L. W. (ed.), Blacks in Southern politics 209–225. New York: Praeger.
Sigelman, C. K, Sigelman, l., Walkosz, B. J., Nitz, M. (1995). Black candidates, white voters: Understanding racial bias in political perceptions. American Journal of Political Science 39, 243–265.
Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Stoll, M. A., & Wong, J. S. (2007). Immigration and civic participation in a multiracial and multiethnic context. International Migration Review, 41(4), 880–908.
Sue, C. A., & Golash-Boza, T. (2013). It was only a joke”: How racial humour fuels colour-blind ideologies in Mexico and Peru. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 36(10), 1582–1598.
Tate, K. (1994). From protest to politics: The new black voters in American elections. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Terkildsen, N. (1993). When white voters evaluate black candidates: The processing implications of candidate skin color, prejudice, and self-monitoring. American Journal of Political Science 37, 1032–1053.
Villarreal, A. (2010). Stratification by skin color in contemporary Mexico. American Sociological Review, 75(5), 652–678.
Wals, S. C. (2013). Made in the USA? Immigrants’ imported ideology and political engagement. Electoral Studies, 32(4), 756–767.
Weaver, V. (2012). The electoral consequences of skin color: The “Hidden” side of race in politics. Political Behavior, 34(1), 159–192.
Winter, N. (2010). Masculine Republicans and feminine Democrats: Gender and Americans’ explicit and implicit images of the political parties. Political Behavior, 32(4), 587–618.
Wong, J. S. (2000). The effects of age and political exposure on the development of party identification among Asian American and Latino immigrants in the United States. Political Behavior, 22(4), 341–371.
Acknowledgements
In addition to the funders mentioned above, we also acknowledge the support of the Center for Comparative Immigration Studies and the Center for U.S.-Mexican Studies. The project could not have been carried out without the efforts of Enrico Marcelli, Allison Van Vooren, Wayne Cornelius, and all of the students who participated in the 2013–2014 round of the Mexican Migration Field Research Program. We thank Seth Hill, Ricardo Ramirez, Tom Wong, Will Terry, and seminar participants at the 2014 MPSA, APSA, and PRIEC meetings for helpful comments. The standard disclaimer applies.
This work was supported by Grants from the AVINA Foundation and the University of Arizona’s Borders Program, whom the authors gratefully acknowledge. Replication materials are available at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/2PAZJX
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
A Appendix
A Appendix
See Appendix Table 2, Figs. 6, 7.
A.1 Randomization Check
In this randomization check, we examine whether there are differences in the observable characteristics of our respondents, grouped by treatment assignment category. If such differences existed, it would be evidence that there was a systematic breakdown of our randomization and assignment regime. No such differences exist, neither as reported here in Table 2, nor in a likelihood ratio test (not reported) in our analysis.
A.2 Ideology Through Time
A possible concern is that the primary driver of our results for differential evaluation of candidate ideology through time might result from different ideology profiles being present among our sample of Mexican-born residents of San Diego County. There is no evidence to suggest an association between SDCMIHLSS participant’s time in the US and reported ideology. As we report in Appendix Fig. 6 neither a split by binned ideology (in the left panel) nor a full recover of the data (in the right panel) give any support to such a theory.
A.3 Knowledge of Party Systems in Mexico
Each of the overlaid density plots shows the distribution of ideology values attributed to the candidate. The values are grouped according to the respondent’s answer to the question, “If [the target candidate] were affiliated with a political party, which party do you think it would be?” In other words, the values plotted in red (medium shade) show how respondents who presumed the candidate belonged to the left-leaning PRD described the candidate’s ideology. The figure shows that, while there is substantial variation in the attributed ideology of the candidate, respondents are generally reliable in classifying the main political parties of Mexico along the ideological spectrum.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Aguilar, R., Hughes, D.A. & Gell-Redman, M. Phenotypic Preference in Mexican Migrants: Evidence from a Random Household Survey. Polit Behav 41, 187–207 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9445-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9445-9