Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Contentious Activities, Disrespectful Protesters: Effect of Protest Context on Protest Support and Mobilization Across Ideology and Authoritarianism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Protest is a tool more often wielded by the political left than right. Somewhat surprisingly, there has been little careful investigation of this asymmetry to date. I show that by examining how the protest context interacts with individual-level differences in ideology and authoritarianism, we gain insight into the protest asymmetry and the dynamics of public protest support and mobilization potential. Using an experimental design, I find that contentious protests and protesters that are disrespectful of police reduce public support, and that liberals and conservatives, and nonauthoritarians and authoritarians, are affected by the protest context in different ways. In my study, conservatives were found to be less supportive than liberals of protests that were disrespectful of police and were demobilized by violent protests. For authoritarians, however, violence did not decrease their support or mobilization. Rather, what decreased support and mobilization among authoritarians were protests that were disrespectful of authorities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See Opp et al. (1995) for their broader theory, which includes party identification and selective incentives. Also see Dalton et al. (2010) for a similar point about the role of party id and political history.

  2. This means that protest is not inherently a liberal or conservative phenomenon, according to Opp et al. (1995), but one in which the meanings and experiences associated with particular ideologies affect individual protest participation.

  3. My results described below hold when controlling for beliefs about the effectiveness of protest and past participation in a variety of actions, including protest.

  4. This is similar to Leeuwen et al. (2015) concept of “atmosphere”—“the affective state that people attribute to the idiosyncratic features of a demonstration” (p. 84). Important to these perceptions are interactions with others, especially interactions between police and demonstrators.

  5. See the online Appendix for the geographic distribution of study participants.

  6. Replication data and Stata syntax files for this project can be found on Dataverse at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/COZN1F.

  7. To my knowledge, no observational dataset is available that would allow me to test my hypotheses.

  8. During the first launch of the study on Mturk there was a mistake in setting the restriction to U.S. workers only, which is how some non-U.S. workers snuck in. The mistake was detected early and rectified immediately.

  9. Participants could not advance past the article until it had been displayed on screen for at least 20 s.

  10. Using Pew Research Center Polling results, these issues were identified as being the most polarized across partisanship in terms of the importance that Democrats and Republicans assigned to these issues. A table summarizing these poll results can be found in the Online Appendix.

  11. Full article wording for the anti-immigration conditions can be found in the Online Appendix.

  12. Note: The issues themselves were not used in random assignment.

  13. Full results in terms of which issues were chosen as most important can be found in the Online Appendix.

  14. Mean levels of authoritarianism using the American National Election studies range from 0.60 in the 2004 ANES to 0.62 in the 2000 and 2012 ANES.

  15. Random assignments checks can be found in the Online Appendix. Results show that random assignment effectively created equivalent groups in terms of gender, race, age, income, ideology, and authoritarianism.

  16. For the proceeding analyses, I pool all the samples and use the combined data, as there were too few conservatives in the student and blog samples to make meaningful comparisons using those samples alone.

  17. Because of the inclusion of the interaction term, the coefficients for each manipulated factor are interpreted as the average difference in peacefulness or respectfulness ratings for each manipulation when the other treatment factor is held at 0.

  18. Figure A2 in the Online Appendix plots these results for ease of comparison.

  19. The following results hold when controlling for a variety of other factors, including issue attitudes, beliefs that protest is an effective political activity, and past political behavior. Results also hold when using ordered probit regression, but for ease of interpretation and presentation, OLS results are presented.

  20. These results are substantively unchanged when looking across individual samples or when dummies for the different samples are included. Mean levels of protest support and protest intentions by experimental condition, including the full and individual samples, are presented in the Appendix. Across all samples, the same pattern of experimental effects emerges.

  21. Results that include dummy variables for each of the conditions and their interactions with ideology and authoritarianism can be found in the Online Appendix. The results are substantively the same and those reported in the main text.

  22. It is important to note that the differences emerged on the conservative end of the spectrum. Liberals were not mobilized by these conditions—the movement was among conservatives who were demobilized by violence above and beyond their relative disinclination towards protest.

  23. See Lipset (1959); Lipset and Raab (1978); Hofstadter (1967); Duncan and Stewart (1995); Duncan 1999; Duncan et al. 1997; Duncan et al. (2010) for work that attempts to examine authoritarians’ political activism.

References

  • Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. Oxford: Harpers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10(10), 1246–1247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., & Mendes, S. M. (2005). Learning to lose: Election outcomes, democratic experience and political protest potential. British Journal of Political Science, 36(1), 91–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. J. (2006). The relationship between right-wing authoritarianism and attitudes toward violence: Further validation of the Attitudes Toward Violence Scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An international journal, 34(8), 923–926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon. com’s Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, H., & Opp, K. (2014). Personality traits (“Big Five”) and the propensity to political protest: Alternative models. Political Psychology, 35(4), 515–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buechler, S. M. (2000). Social movements in advanced capitalism. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. C. (2000). Personality and emotional correlates of right-wing authoritarianism. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 28(1), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind.”. Political Psychology, 29(6), 807–840.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassese, E. C., Huddy, L., Hartman, T. K., Mason, L., & Weber, C. R. (2013). Socially mediated internet surveys: Recruiting participants for online experiments. PS: Political Science & Politics, 46(4), 775–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In David Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self- identifications. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, R. (2002). Citizen politics: Public opinion and political parties in advanced industrial democracies (3rd ed.). London: Chatham House Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, R. (2013). Citizen politics: Public opinion and political parties in advanced industrial democracies. Washington, DC: Cq Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, R., Van Sickle, A., & Weldon, S. (2010). The individual–institutional nexus of protest behaviour. British Journal of Political Science, 40(1), 51–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doty, R. M., Peterson, B. E., & Winter, D. (1991). Threat and authoritarianism in the United States, 1978–1987. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(4), 629–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckitt, J. (1989). Authoritarianism and group identification: A new perspective of an old construct. Political Psychology, 10, 63–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. E. (1999). Motivation for collective action: Group consciousness as mediator of personality, life experiences, and women’s rights activism. Political Psychology, 20(3), 611–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. E., Peterson, B. E., & Winter, D. G. (1997). Authoritarianism and gender roles: Toward a psychological analysis of hegemonic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(1), 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. E., Peterson, B. E., & Zurbriggen, E. L. (2010). Personality and politics: Introduction to the special issue. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1595–1600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. E., & Stewart, A. J. (1995). Still bringing the Vietnam War home: Sources of contemporary student activism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(9), 914–924.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Kovacheff, C. (2017). Extreme protest tactics reduce popular support for social movements. Unpublished manuscript. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2911177.

  • Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24(1), 41–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 18(4), 741–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamson, W. A. (2004). Bystanders, public opinion, and the media. In D. A. Snow, S. A. Soule, & H. Kriesi (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to social movements (pp. 242–261). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Personality and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts. American Political Science Review, 104(01), 111–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giugni, M., McAdam, D., & Tilly, C. (Eds.). (1999). How social movements matter. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and Polarization in American Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M., & Suhay, E. (2011). Authoritarianism, threat, and Americans’ support for the war on terror. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 546–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 297–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, E. L. (1990). Sacrifice for the cause: Group processes, recruitment, and commitment in a student social movement. American Sociological Review, pp. 243–254.

  • Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and political change in 43 societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D., & Simas, E. N. (2010). Risk orientations and policy frames. The Journal of Politics, 72(2), 381–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerpelman, L. C. (1969). Student political activism and ideology: Comparative characteristics of activists and nonactivists. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 16(1).

  • Klandermans, B., & Mayer, N. (Eds.). (2005). Extreme right activists in Europe. Through the magnifying glass. London and New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klandermans, B., & Van Steekelenberg, J. (2013). Social movements and the dynamics of collective action. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. Levy (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, D., & Klingemann, H.-D. (1990). The left-right schema. In M. K. Jennings & J. W. Van Deth (Eds.), Continuities in political action: A longitudinal study of political orientations in three western democracies (Vol. 5, pp. 203–234). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linden, A., & Klandermans, B. (2006). Stigmatization and repression of extreme-right activism in the Netherlands. Mobilization: An International Quarterly, 11(2), 213–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipset, S. M. (1959). Democracy and working-class authoritarianism. American Sociological Review, 24, 482–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipset, S. M., & Raab, E. (1978). The politics of unreason: Right-wing extremism in America, 1790–1977 (Vol. 5). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 323.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, D. M. (1995). Communicating deviance: The effects of television news coverage of social protest. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 39(1), 4–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, D. M., & Hertog, J. K. (1992). The manufacture of public opinion by reporters: Informal cues for public perceptions of protest groups. Discourse & Society, 3(3), 259–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. S., & Tarrow, S. G. (1998). The social movement society: Contentious politics for a new century. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Opp, K., Finkel, S. E., Muller, E. N., Wolfsfeld, G., Dietz, H., & Green, J. (1995). Left-right ideology and collective political action: A comparative analysis of Germany, Israel, and Peru. In C. J. Jenkins (Ed.), The politics of social protest: comparative perspectives on states and social movements (pp. 63–95). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen Reports. (2015). Most see baltimore riots as criminal, not protest. Retrieved November 20, 2017 from http://www.rasmussenreports.com/public_content/politics/general_politics/april_2015/most_see_baltimore_riots_as_criminal_not_protest.

  • Schussman, A., & Soule, S. A. (2005). Process and protest: Accounting for individual protest participation. Social Forces, 84(2), 1083–1108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). Academic Press.

  • Soule, S. A., & Earl, J. (2005). A movement society evaluated: Collective protest in the United States, 1960–1986. Mobilization: An International Quarterly, 10(3), 345–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T. D., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Human ecology review, 6(2), 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarrow, S. G. (2011). Power in movement: Social movements and contentious politics. Cambridge University Press.

  • Thorisdottir, H., Jost, J. T., Liviatan, I., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). Psychological needs and values underlying left-right political orientation: Cross-national evidence from Eastern and Western Europe. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(2), 175–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Meer, T., Van Deth, J., & Scheepers, P. (2009). The politicized participant: Ideology and political action in 20 democracies. Comparative Political Studies, 42(11), 1426–1457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hiel, A., Kossowska, M., & Mervielde, I. (2000). The relationship between openness to experience and political ideology. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(4), 741–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Leeuwen, A., Klandermans, B., & van Stekelenburg, J. (2015). A study of perceived protest atmospheres: How demonstrators evaluate police-demonstrator interactions and why. An International Quarterly, 20(1), 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velez, Y. R., & Lavine, H. (2017). Racial diversity and the dynamics of authoritarianism. The Journal of Politics, 79(2), 519–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., Brady, H., & Nie, N. H. (1993). Citizen activity: Who participates? What do they say?. American Political Science Review, 87(2), 303–318.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would first and foremost like to thank Leonie Huddy for her guidance on this project. I am also grateful to Patrick Lown and colleagues from Stony Brook University and the University of Essex for their feedback on earlier drafts of this article. I would also like to thank Gelly for her ever-present support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Raynee Sarah Gutting.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 637 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gutting, R.S. Contentious Activities, Disrespectful Protesters: Effect of Protest Context on Protest Support and Mobilization Across Ideology and Authoritarianism. Polit Behav 42, 865–890 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-09523-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-09523-8

Keywords

Navigation