Skip to main content
Log in

Electoral Confidence, Overconfidence, and Risky Behavior: Evidence from a Study with Elected Politicians

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Democratic theory makes strong assumptions about the relationship between politicians’ likelihood of retaining office and their behavior in office. Specifically, confidence in re-election is often used to explain a willingness to take risks. In this paper, we make a distinction between politicians’ accurate assessments of their likelihood of being re-elected and an overestimation of this likelihood (i.e. their overconfidence). We argue that overconfidence by politicians is associated with a higher willingness to make risky decisions. Using a sample of incumbent members of the national parliaments of Belgium, Canada, and Israel, we show that their preference for risk-taking is predicted by self-reported confidence in their likelihood of re-election. We further show that this relationship is largely explained by overconfidence, while ‘objective’ electoral safety is not associated with risky behavior in office.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The literature is vast. Notable examples include Downs and Rocke (1994), Hood (2002), Jones (2001) Levy (2003) McDermott (2001), Vis (2010).

  2. A full review is impractical, but see for example March and Olsen (1995), Przeworski et al. (1999), Weaver (1986).

  3. Here we report results based on within-elite differences in overconfidence levels. Whether politicians as a group are indeed more prone to general displays of overconfidence relative to non-politicians is beyond the scope of this study, principally because we are interested in overconfidence with respect to electoral performance, which we cannot evaluate with non-politicians. Nevertheless, we do know that politicians are consistently more risk-taking relative to citizens when asked to make similar policy choices (Sheffer et al. 2017).

  4. One objection to collecting evaluations from individual politicians on their risk preferences is that in practice they do not affect in-office decision-making (and the resultant policies), because partisan voting and coalition/opposition sorting wash them out. This critique, we believe, misses many of the instances in which politicians’ individual-level preferences and traits matter for in-office behavior: politicians have a greater degree of independence in committees, in internal party debates, and when they interact with constituents. Individual differences also matter more when decisions are made (or positions are debated) on non-salient issues, new issues, or those that do not easily break according to partisan lines—such as public health crises like the one simulated in the task we administered. Beyond that, a concentration of a critical mass of politicians with specific behavioral preferences within a party, or having a party leader or cabinet minister with certain preferences can have a direct impact on the adoption of those party/government positions that are then enforced in parliament. In short, we believe that individual-level decision-making characteristics of politicians are highly consequential, and warrant direct examination.

  5. This study is part of an ERC-funded multinational research project called Infopol that involved in-person interviews with several hundred incumbent politicians. For more details: www.infopol-project.org.

  6. The Pearson correlation coefficients for overconfidence with gain/loss frame assignment and accountability treatment assignment are 0.01 and 0.11, respectively, and neither is significantly different from zero. The same correlations for electoral safety are − 0.01 and − 0.13, and again neither is significantly different from zero.

  7. We also report the treatment effects on our sample in the Online Appendix. Results of the treatment effects from this experiment are reported fully in another study [OMITTED]. We find, similar to Linde and Vis (2017), that politicians exhibit a strong preference reversal effect when moving between the gain/loss frames. Priming different accountability levels does not significantly affect risk seeking.

  8. For example, Rubenzer et al. (2000) adopt this approach in evaluating the personalities of US presidents.

  9. Including for the MPs in our sample; see Table 11 in the Online Appendix.

  10. Some studies (e.g. Bengtsson 2004, see also Lewis-Beck and Stegmaier 2013, pp. 378–379) show that in certain electoral systems, particularly those where coalition governments are common, conditions arise where past vote share is negatively correlated with future electoral performance, due to a cyclical pattern of governing party punishment. This argument concerns overall party support, and it is unclear if and to what degree it applies to individual MPs’ likelihood of re-election.

  11. An alternative operationalization using the MP’s absolute vote proportion in the riding rather than the distance from the runner-up yields essentially identical results.

  12. The results remain substantively identical when estimating linear regression models instead.

  13. This figure relates to the raw frequency across our sample. When computing predicted probabilities over the treatments, the overall predicted rate is 60%; see Online Appendix for full details.

  14. We include plots of the same relationships using raw risk-taking in the Online Appendix. They exhibit the same trends.

  15. With respect to the over-correlation risk in multiple testing, we stress that we were theoretically motivated and did not conduct additional tests on other measures that were collected. Moreover, the Asian Disease vignette was the only behavioral assessment task in this survey, and in any case the only module that we theoretically expected to have a relationship with confidence in re-election. In that sense, we are not under-reporting or omitting other tests from the analysis presented here. With respect to the risk of over-correlation as a result of shared error terms between variables, we acknowledge the inherent difficulty in overcoming it in one-shot survey designs of the kind we conduct, which is particularly difficult to overcome in the context of surveying difficult to re-contact elites. We further note a recommendation by Bullock et al. (2010), who suggest examining effects among different groups of subjects as a way of substantiating aggregate correlations, so that if “these effects differ little from group to group (e.g., from women to men, authoritarians to nonauthoritarians), we become more confident that causal heterogeneity is not affecting our analysis.” (p. 555) Indeed, we do this by looking at by-country subgroups, and we find similar patterns, which, while not reducing the risk of over-estimation, give us at least some confidence that the correlation we are seeing is not inflated to the degree of creating a false positive.

References

  • Alesina, A. (1988). Macroeconomics and politics. NBER Macroeconomics Annual, 3, 13–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alesina, A. (1993). A model of the political economy of the united states. American Political Science Review, 87(1), 12–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D. R., & Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, R. D. (1992). The logic of congressional action. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arvey, R. D., Rotundo, M., Johnson, W., Zhang, Z., & McGue, M. (2006). The determinants of leadership role occupancy: Genetic and personality factors. The Leadership Quarterly, 17(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashworth, S. (2012). Electoral accountability: Recent theoretical and empirical work. Annual Review of Political Science, 15, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bengtsson, Å. (2004). Economic voting: The effect of political context, volatility and turnout on voters assignment of responsibility. European Journal of Political Research, 43(5), 749–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, H. (2011). Does personality matter in politics? Personality factors as determinants of parliamentary recruitment and policy preferences. Comparative Sociology, 10(6), 928–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broockman, D. E., & Skovron, C. (2013). What politicians believe about their constituents: Asymmetric misperceptions and prospects for constituency control. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but whats the mechanism? (dont expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental approach. The American Economic Review, 89(1), 306–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, W. K., Goodie, A. S., & Foster, J. D. (2004). Narcissism, confidence, and risk attitude. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(4), 297–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canes-Wrone, B., Herron, M. C., & Shotts, K. W. (2001). Leadership and pandering: A theory of executive policymaking. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 532–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Consiglio, C., Picconi, L., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Personalities of politicians and voters: Unique and synergistic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnevale, J. J., Inbar, Y., & Lerner, J. S. (2011). Individual differences in need for cognition and decision-making competence among leaders. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 274–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiozza, G., & Goemans, H. E. (2004). International conflict and the tenure of leaders: Is war still ex post inefficient? American Journal of Political Science, 48(3), 604–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., & Subrahmanyam, A. (1998). Investor psychology and security market under-and overreactions. The Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1839–1885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Mesquita, B. B., & Siverson, R. M. (1995). War and the survival of political leaders: A comparative study of regime types and political accountability. American Political Science Review, 89(04), 841–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, M., & Ortoleva, P. (2015). Is it all connected? A testing ground for unified theories of behavioral economics phenomena. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2643355.

  • Downs, G. W., & Downs, D. M. (1994). Conflict, agency, and gambling for resurrection: The principal-agent problem goes to war. American Journal of Political Science, 38, 362–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drazen, A. (2000). The political business cycle after 25 years. NBER Macroeconomics Annual, 15, 75–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98(04), 671–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2012). Experimenting with politics. Science, 335(6073), 1177–1179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enos, R. D., & Hersh, E. D. (2015). Campaign perceptions of electoral closeness: Uncertainty, fear and over-confidence. British Journal of Political Science, 47, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, R. S. (1990). Economic conditions and the congressional vote: A review of the macrolevel evidence. American Journal of Political Science, 34(2), 373–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenno, R. F. (1978). Home style: House members in their districts. New York: Pearson College Division.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. L., & Lawless, J. L. (2005). To run or not to run for office: Explaining nascent political ambition. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 642–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., & King, G. (1990). Estimating incumbency advantage without bias. American Journal of Political Science, 34, 1142–1164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2012). Field experiments: Design, analysis, and interpretation. New York: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grose, C. R. (2013). Risk and roll calls: How legislators’ personal finances shape congressional decisions. USC CLASS Research Paper No. CLASS13-7, 13–20.

  • Hafner-Burton, E. M., Haggard, S., Lake, D. A., & Victor, D. G. (2017). The behavioral revolution and international relations. International Organization, 71(S1), S1–S31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hafner-Burton, E. M., Hughes, D. A., & Victor, D. G. (2013). The cognitive revolution and the political psychology of elite decision making. Perspectives on Politics, 11(02), 368–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, A., & Malhotra, N. (2009). Myopic voters and natural disaster policy. American Political Science Review, 103(03), 387–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, A., & Malhotra, N. (2013). Retrospective voting reconsidered. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 285–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M. (2002). Deciding analytically or trusting your intuition? the advantages and disadvantages of analytic and intuitive thought. UPF Economics and Business Working Paper No. 654. Retrieved from SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=394920 or https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.394920.

  • Hood, C. (2002). The risk game and the blame game. Government and opposition, 37(01), 15–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilies, R., Arvey, R. D., & Bouchard, T. J. (2006). Darwinism, behavioral genetics, and organizational behavior: A review and agenda for future research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(2), 121–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D. (2009). Overconfidence and war. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. D. (2001). Politics and the architecture of choice: Bounded rationality and governance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, R. A., Sellers, J. G., Newman, M. L., & Mehta, P. H. (2006). The mismatch effect: When testosterone and status are at odds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(6), 999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Kahneman, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 47(2), 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingdon, J. W. (1989). Congressmen’s voting decisions. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koellinger, P., Minniti, M., & Schade, C. (2007). I think i can, i think i can: Overconfidence and entrepreneurial behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(4), 502–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, S., & Méndez, E. (2005). Policy makers’ preferences, party ideology, and the political business cycle. Southern Economic Journal, 71(4), 752–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kühberger, A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75(1), 23–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. S. (2003). Applications of prospect theory to political science. Synthese, 135(2), 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis-Beck, M. S., & Stegmaier, M. (2013). The vp-function revisited: A survey of the literature on vote and popularity functions after over 40 years. Public Choice, 157(3–4), 367–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, S.-W., & Bier, V. M. (2008). A study of expert overconfidence. Reliability Engineering & System Safety, 93(5), 711–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linde, J., & Vis, B. (2017). Do politicians take risks like the rest of us? An experimental test of prospect theory under MPS. Political Psychology, 38(1), 101–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., Jones, B. C., & Roberts, S. C. (2007). Facial appearance affects voting decisions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(1), 18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X., Stoutenborough, J., & Vedlitz, A. (2016). Bureaucratic expertise, overconfidence, and policy choice. Governance, 30(4), 705–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malmendier, U., & Tate, G. (2008). Who makes acquisitions? CEO overconfidence and the market’s reaction. Journal of Financial Economics, 89(1), 20–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • March, J. G., & Olsen, J. P. (1995). Democratic governance. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. (2001). Risk-taking in international politics: Prospect theory in American foreign policy. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, A., Redd, S. B., & Vedlitz, A. (2006). Can we generalize from student experiments to the real world in political science, military affairs, and international relations? Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50(5), 757–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. A., & Healy, P. J. (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review, 115(2), 502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, N., Soane, E., Fenton-O’Creevy, M., & Willman, P. (2005). Personality and domain-specific risk taking. Journal of Risk Research, 8(2), 157–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odean, T. (1998). Volume, volatility, price, and profit when all traders are above average. The Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1887–1934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, J. E., & Ha, S. E. (2007). Vote choice in suburban elections. American Political Science Review, 101(03), 393–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ortoleva, P., & Snowberg, E. (2015). Overconfidence in political behavior. The American Economic Review, 105(2), 504–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Przeworski, A., Stokes, S. C., & Manin, B. (1999). Democracy, accountability, and representation (Vol. 2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramey, A., Klingler, J., & Hollibaugh, G. E. (2015). Talking heads: Measuring elite personality using speech. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2605644.

  • Ramey, A. J., Klingler, J. D., & Hollibaugh, G. E. (2016). Measuring elite personality using speech. Political Science Research and Methods. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2016.12

  • Rubenzer, S. J., Faschingbauer, T. R., & Ones, D. S. (2000). Assessing the US presidents using the revised NEO personality inventory. Assessment, 7(4), 403–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, P. S., Williams, C. C., Goodie, A. S., & Campbell, W. K. (2004). Overconfidence and the big five. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(5), 473–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheffer, L., Loewen, P., Walgrave, S., Soroka, S., & Shaefer, T. (2017). Non-representative representatives: An experimental study of the decision making of elected politicians. American Political Science Review (forthcoming).

  • Simon, M., & Houghton, S. M. (2003). The relationship between overconfidence and the introduction of risky products: Evidence from a field study. Academy of Management Journal, 46(2), 139–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spisak, B. R., Homan, A. C., Grabo, A., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Facing the situation: Testing a biosocial contingency model of leadership in intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(2), 273–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stankov, L., & Crawford, J. D. (1997). Self-confidence and performance on tests of cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 25(2), 93–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 1623–1626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyszka, T., & Zielonka, P. (2002). Expert judgments: Financial analysts versus weather forecasters. The Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 3(3), 152–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M. (2006). Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 354–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, M., & Ronay, R. (2014). The evolutionary psychology of leadership theory, review, and roadmap. Organizational Psychology Review, 4(1), 74–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vis, B. (2010). Politics of risk-taking: Welfare state reform in advanced democracies. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, R. K. (1986). The politics of blame avoidance. Journal of Public Policy, 6(04), 371–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Z., Ilies, R., & Arvey, R. D. (2009). Beyond genetic explanations for leadership: The moderating role of the social environment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(2), 118–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Stefaan Walgrave, Stuart Soroka, Tamir Sheafer, Eran Amsalem, Matthew Ayling, Yves Dejaeghere, Lynn Epping, Jeroen Joly, Yogev Karasenty, Julie Sevenans, Tal Shahaf, Kirsten Van Camp, Debby Vos, and Alon Zoizner for their work on this project; the editor and three anonymous reviewers for their thorough and helpful feedback; participants of the 2017 University of Notre Dame Conference on Elite Personality and Political Institutions, the 2017 Midwest Political Science Association and Southern Political Science Association conferences, and the 2015 NYU Abu Dhabi Workshop on Behavioural Models of Politics. This work was supported by the European Research Council [Advanced Grant ‘INFOPOL’, 295735] and the Research Fund of the University of Antwerp [Grant 26827].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lior Sheffer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 510 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sheffer, L., Loewen, P. Electoral Confidence, Overconfidence, and Risky Behavior: Evidence from a Study with Elected Politicians. Polit Behav 41, 31–51 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9438-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9438-0

Keywords

Navigation