Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Partisanship and Preference Formation: Competing Motivations, Elite Polarization, and Issue Importance

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An enduring and increasingly acute concern—in an age of polarized parties—is that people’s partisan attachments distort preference formation at the expense of relevant information. For example, research suggests that a Democrat may support a policy proposed by Democrats, but oppose the same policy if proposed by Republicans. However, a related body of literature suggests that how people respond to information and form preferences is distorted by their prior issue attitudes. In neither instance is information even-handedly evaluated, rather, it is interpreted in light of partisanship or existing issue opinions. Both effects are well documented in isolation, but in most political scenarios individuals consider both partisanship and prior opinions—yet, these dynamics may or may not pull toward the same preference. Using nationally representative experiments focused on tax and education policies, I introduce and test a theory that isolates when: partisanship dominates preference formation, partisanship and issue opinions reinforce or offset each other, and issue attitudes trump partisanship. The findings make clear that the public does not blindly follow party elites. Depending on elite positions, the level of partisan polarization, and personal importance of issues, the public can be attentive to information and shirk the influence of party elites. The results have broad implications for political parties and citizen competence in contemporary democratic politics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. People also possess “accuracy” motivations—a drive to make the “correct” decision (Kunda 1990).

  2. This process is related to but distinct from heuristic cue-taking (Petersen et al. 2013).

  3. Polarization may also heighten perceptions of threat that prompt in-group solidarity and negative out-group prejudices (Huddy et al. 2005). Partisans are also more likely to exert effort to defend beliefs when the opposing party is viewed as competitive (Matthews 2013).

  4. This is distinct from dual-process models of reasoning (Petty and Cacioppo 1986; Petty and Wegener 1999). Dual-process models such as the ELM suggest that attitude change occurs through “central” and “peripheral” routes. Central-route attitude change are based on “effortful information processing aimed at scrutinizing and uncovering the central merits of the issue,” while the peripheral route relies on less cognitive processes such as heuristics (Petty and Wegener 1999). The theory of intersecting motivations outlined here differs in at least a couple ways. Issue-motivated reasoning often leads people to engage in effortful information processing, but this is done so as to defend one’s prior attitudes, not to uncover the merits of the issue as in central processing. Further, partisan motivated reasoning need not be seen as heuristic processing (Petersen et al. 2013), since party cues activate motivational processes that require effortful strategies such as counter arguing incongruent messages.

  5. Leeper and Slothuus (2014) posit, “motivated reasoning will look differently for individuals depending on what issues are at stake and how intensely they need to defend their prior attitudes or identities” (28). Carsey and Layman (2006) also suggest that the relationship between issue positions and partisanship is moderated by issue importance. That the outcome of conflicting motivations is contingent on the salience of motivations is akin to Converse’s (1964) discussion of “centrality.” That is, if the policy attitude is in conflict with the party position but the attitude is less central, the policy attitude will be updated to reflect the party position.

  6. As discussed below, I avoid these assumptions with an alternative experimental design that measures people’s priors in a follow-up study.

  7. Research Now implemented the survey via the Internet with a representative sample of the U.S. adult population. Percent Democrat (36.88), percent Republican (31.51), percent liberal (33.98), percent conservative (36.66), median age category (35–50), percent female (56.06), percent white (84.54), and median income category ($30,000–69,999). The sample employs an opt-in panel that acquires potential respondents from various partners (e.g., commercial sponsors). They invite individuals to join the survey panel and financially compensate them for participation. For each survey, ResearchNow (2015, p. 2) employs “a proprietary method of exclusively inviting pre-validated individuals, or individuals who share known characteristics…” That is, they employ an algorithm based on response rates to ensure a sample that is representative of the U.S. population, based on U.S. census figures. The company also implements a wide range of data quality validation checks, including checking for logical answers, consistent reporting, minimal non-response, realistic response times, etc. (for general discussion of these types of samples, see, e.g., Callegaro et al. 2014).

  8. See Supporting Information for power analyses.

  9. This ensures the first issue is uncontaminated. Randomizing issue order allows for spillover effects.

  10. Certainly, participants come into the experiment with pre-existing levels of personal issue importance. Yet, due to random assignment, similar proportions of high/low issue importance individuals should be found in each experimental condition. However, I am primarily concerned with average treatment effects, and, as such, I simply need the manipulation to alter personal issue importance on average for most participants—which was confirmed by pre-tests. An alternative approach would be to explore whether preference formation is moderated by people’s existing personal issue importance (i.e., observational survey data analysis), but here I opt to experimentally manipulate issue importance in an effort to wield greater causal leverage.

  11. Data and code for replication are available on the Political Behavior Dataverse webpage. Because hypotheses are directional in nature, one-tailed p values and 90 % confidence intervals are reported except when otherwise noted. As evident by p values, most results remain significant if two-tailed tests are employed. I also reference results from ANOVA tests with Bonferroni corrections to account for multiple group comparisons.

  12. ANOVA results for multiple group comparison of when motivations pull in same directions: Democrats tax issue F(4, 542) = 168.09, p < 0.001. Bonferroni multiple comparison test reveals significant differences (p < 0.001) between all groups except conditions 3 and 4. Democrats education issue F(4, 528) = 98.20, (p < 0.001). Bonferroni test reveals significant differences (p < 0.01) between all groups except conditions 3 and 4. Republicans tax issue F(4, 439) = 58.28, (p < 0.001). Bonferroni test reveals significant differences (p < 0.02) between all group except 3 and 4, 3 and 5, and 5 and 6. Republicans education issue F(4, 426) = 27.59, (p < 0.001). Bonferroni test reveals significant differences (p < 0.02) between all groups except 3 and 4, 3 and 5 (p < 0.065), 4 and 6, and 5 and 6.

  13. When analyses of both issues are broken down by political knowledge (see Supporting Information), there are few differences between low, moderate, and high knowledge respondents. The one exception is high knowledge Democrats in the education issue. These individuals reveal little movement across conditions on the education issue, and may, in part, be driving the null effect of the reversed party endorsement.

  14. Because hypothesis is not directional, two-tailed tests of significance are employed here.

  15. ANOVA results for multiple group comparison of when motivations compete: Republicans tax issue F(4, 463) = 78.11, p < 0.001. Bonferroni multiple comparison test reveals significant differences (p < 0.001) between all groups except conditions 7 and 9, and 1 and 10 (expected). Republicans education issue F(4, 445) = 47.95, p < 0.001. Bonferroni test reveals significant differences (p < 0.01) between all groups except conditions 1 and 10 (expected). Democrats tax issue F(4, 497) = 14.71, p < 0.001. Bonferroni test reveals significant differences (p < 0.01) between all groups except 1 and 10 (expected), 1 and 9, 7 and 9, 7 and 10, and 9 and 10. Democrats education issue F(4, 483) = 4.27, p < 0.003. Bonferroni test reveals significant differences only between groups 8 and 9 (p < 0.001).

  16. People were also asked, post-stimuli, about the importance of their partisan identification to determine if the polarization and issue importance prompts shape the salience of partisanship as intended. The results are shown in the Supporting Information and reveal two patterns consistent with expectations. First, the high polarization prompts increase the importance of party identification. Second, the high issue importance cues which seems to encourage respondents to focus on substantive information, and in doing so, reduces the importance of partisanship.

  17. Due to the small sample size and the preponderance of Democratic respondents, Republicans do not afford sufficient statistical power for analysis. Focusing only on Democrats who initially oppose the proposal (i.e. take the traditional party position) I mitigate assumptions in the previous experimental design about people’s prior attitudes.

References

  • Abramowitz, A. I. (2010). The disappearing center: engaged citizens, polarization, and American democracy. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Political Science Association. (1950). Toward a more responsible two-party system: A report of the committee on political parties. American Political Science Review, 44, 1–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anand, S., & Krosnick, J. A. (2003). The impact of attitudes toward foreign policy goals on public preferences among presidential candidates: A study of issue publics and the attentive public in the 2000 U.S. Presidential Election. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 33, 31–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apsler, R., & Sears, D. O. (1968). Warning, personal involvement, and attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 162–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K. (2008). Can partisan cues diminish democratic accountability? Political Behavior, 30(2), 139–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., & Vander Wielen, R. J. (2013). The effects of need for cognition and need for affect on partisan evaluations. Political Psychology, 34(1), 23–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berelson, B. (1952). Democratic theory and public opinion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 16(3), 313–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bisgaard, M. (2015). Bias will find a way: Economic perceptions, attributions of blame, and partisan motivated reasoning during a crisis. The Journal of Politics, 77(3), 849–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohman, J. (1996). Public deliberation. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute for Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., & Cook, F. L. (2014). The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Political Behavior, 36, 235–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T., & Leeper, T. J. (2013). Self-interest and attention to news among issue publics. Political Communication, 30(3), 329–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boninger, D. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Berent, M. K. (1995). Origins of attitude importance: Self-interest, social identification, and value relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 61–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., & MacKenzie, S. A. (2014). Informing the electorate? How party cues and policy information affect public opinion about initiatives. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 48–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, J. G. (2011). Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. American Political Science Review, 105(3), 496–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, J. G., Gerber, A. S., Hill, S. J., & Huber, G. A. (2013). Partisan bias in factual beliefs about politics. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series.

  • Callegaro, M., Baker, R., Bethlehem, J., Göritz, A. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Lavrakas, P. J. (2014). Online panel research: History, concepts, applications, and a look at the future. In M. Callegaro, R. Baker, J. Bethlehem, A. S. Göritz, J. A. Krosnick, & P. J. Lavrakas (Eds.), Online panel research: A data quality perspetive. West Sussex: Wiley.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cambell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, Donald E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmines, E. G., Ensley, M. J., & Wagner, M. W. (2012). Political ideology in American politics: One, two, or none? The Forum, 10(4), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carsey, T. M., & Layman, G. C. (2006). Changing sides or changing minds? Party identification and policy preferences in the american electorate. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 464–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., Citrin, J., & Conley, P. (2001). When self-interest matters. Political Psychology, 22(3), 541–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciuk, D. J., & Yost, B. (2015). The effects of issue salience, elite influence, and policy on public opinion. Political Communication.

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). 2006 The nature of belief systems in mass publics. Critical Review: A Journal of Politics and Society, 18, 1–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dancey, L., & Goren, P. (2010). Party identification, issue attitudes, and the dynamics of political debate. American Journal of Political Science, 54(July), 686–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1927). The public and its problems. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Disch, L. (2010). Toward a mobilization conception of democratic representation. American Political Science Review, 105(1), 100–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2012). The politics of motivation. Critical Review, 24, 199–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2014). Pathologies of studying public opinion, political communication, and democratic responsiveness. Political Communication, 31, 467–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107, 57–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorina, M. P., & Abrams, S. J. (2008). Political polarization in the American public. Annual Review of Political Science, 11, 563–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Peyton, B., & Verkuilen, J. (2007). Same facts, different interpretations: Partisan motivation and opinion on Iraq. The Journal of Politics, 69(4), 957–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren, P. (2002). Character weakness, partisan bias, and presidential evaluation. American Journal of Political Science, 46(3), 627–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren, P., Federico, C. M., & Kittilson, M. C. (2009). Source cues, partisan identities, and political value expression. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 805–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groenendyk, E. W. (2013). Competing motives in the partisan mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (2006). Political communication in media society: Does democracy still enjoy an epistemic dimension? The impact of normative theory on empirical research. Communication Theory, 16(4), 411–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Highton, B., & Kam, C. D. (2011). The long-term dynamics of partisanship and issue orientations. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 202–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Krosnick, J. K., Visser, P. S., & Boninger, D. S. (2005). Attitude importance and the accumulation of attitude-relevant knowledge in memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 749–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support for antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Walker, John. (2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. The Journal of Politics, 70(1), 186–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, W. G. (1988). The impact of party identification on issue attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 32(3), 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jerit, J. (2007). Issue publics, news interest, and the information environment. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago, IL.

  • Jerit, J., & Barabas, J. (2012). Partisan perceptual bias and the information environment. The Journal of Politics, 74(3), 672–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. M. (2007). How intrinsic and extrinsic motivations interact in selectivity: Investigating the moderating effects of situational information processing goals in issue publics’ web behavior. Communication Research, 34(2), 185–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. M. (2009). Issue publics in the new information environment: Selectivity, domain specificity, and extremity. Communication Research, 36(2), 254–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klar, S. (2014). Partisanship in a social setting. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 687–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. (1990). Government policy and citizen passion: A study of issue publics in contemporary America. Political Behavior, 12, 59–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., Berent, M., & Boninger, D. (1994). Pockets of responsibility in the American electorate: findings of a research program on attitude importance. Political Communication, 11, 391–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lascher, E. L., & Korey, J. L. (2011). The myth of the independent voter, california style. California Journal of Politics and Policy, 3, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and Disadvantages of Cognitive Heuristics in Political Decision Making. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 951–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, H., Johnston, C., & Steenbergen, M. (2012). The ambivalent partisan: How critical loyalty promotes democracy. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lebo, M. J., & Cassino, D. (2007). The aggregated consequences of motivated reasoning and the dynamics of partisan approval. Political Psychology, 28(6), 719–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecheler, S., de Vreese, C., & Slothuus, R. (2009). Issue importance as a moderator of framing effects. Communication Research, 36(3), 400–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T. J. (2014). the informational basis for mass polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 78(1), 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2014). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public opinion formation. Advances in Political Psychology, 35(Supplement 1), 129–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levendusky, M. S. (2009). The partisan sort: How liberal became democrats and conservatives became republicans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Levendusky, M. S. (2010). Clearer cues, more consistent voters: A benefit of elite polarization. Political Behavior, 32, 111–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2013). The rationalizing voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magleby, D. B., Nelson, C. J., & Westlye, M. C. (2011). The myth of the independent voter revisited. In P. M. Sniderman & B. Highton (Eds.), Facing the challenge of democracy: Explorations in the analysis of public opinion and political participation (pp. 238–263). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, J. S. (2013). The electoral basis of partisan motivated reasoning. In Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association Annual Conference, Chicago, IL.

  • Mill, J. S. (1859). On Liberty. In John Gray (Ed.), John Stuart Mill on liberty and other essays, 1998 (pp. 1–128). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullinix, K. J. (2015). Presidential debates, partisan motivations, and political interest. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 45(2), 270–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, M. B., Skov, M., Serrtizlew, S., & Ramsoy, T. (2013). Motivated reasoning and political parties: Evidence for increased processing in the face of party cues. Political Behavior, 35(4), 831–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–72). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, W. M. (1993). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(2), 472–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rich, M. (July 23, 2013). Education overhaul faces a test of partisanship. The New York Times.

  • Robison, J., & Mullinix, K. J. (2015) Elite polarization and public opinion: How polarization is communicated and its effects. Political Communication.

  • Schattschneider, E. E. (1942). Party government. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, R. Y. (2011). Public opinion and American democracy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(5), 982–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slothuus, R., & de Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and issue framing effects. Journal of Politics, 72(3), 630–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. W. (January 24, 2013). Governors push bigger reliance on sales taxes. The New York Times.

  • Taber, C. S., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31, 137–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treier, S., & Hillygus, D. S. (2009). The nature of political ideology in the contemporary electorate. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 679–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., Bizer, G. Y., & Krosnick, J. A. (2006). Exploring the latent structure of strength-related attitude attributes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 1–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Norris, C. M. (2004). Challenging the common-factor model of strength-related attitude attributes: Contrasting the antecedents and consequences of attitude importance and attitude-relevant knowledge. Unpublished Manuscript, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL.

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Jamie Druckman, Yanna Krupnikov, Georgia Kernell, D.J. Flynn, Thomas Leeper, and the Northwestern University Political Parties Working Group for their feedback on earlier drafts of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin J. Mullinix.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 339 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

See Fig. 5.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Bars show mean effectiveness rating of the argument opposed to the tax proposal. Lines represent confidence intervals

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mullinix, K.J. Partisanship and Preference Formation: Competing Motivations, Elite Polarization, and Issue Importance. Polit Behav 38, 383–411 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9318-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9318-4

Keywords

Navigation