Abstract
In this paper, we examine how the relationship between authoritarianism and partisanship is conditioned by education. Arguing against perspectives suggesting that authoritarianism is the province of the “unsophisticated,” we hypothesize that the relationship between education and Republican partisanship should be stronger among educated Americans. Moreover, going beyond previous work on how education may moderate the political impact of psychological dispositions, we also argue that partisan signals pertinent to authoritarianism come more strongly from the right, producing a pattern in which the positive relationship between authoritarianism and preference for the Republican Party over independence is stronger among the educated but the negative relationship between authoritarianism and preference for the Democratic Party over independence is not stronger among the educated. Data from the 2004 and 2008 American National Election Studies indicate clear support for both hypotheses.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
In supplementary analyses not shown here, all of the models reported below were also re-run with an additional interaction between religiosity and biblical literalism included; this was done to explore the possibility that religiosity predicts partisanship more among the religiously traditional. These interactions were not significant, and their inclusion did not change any of our primary results.
In supplementary analyses, two-way Beliefs about Government × College Degree interactions are added to the models in Table 1. This specification allows the effect of beliefs about government as well as the effect of authoritarianism to vary across education levels. Although these interactions were significant in both 2004 and 2008 (ps < 0.001), their inclusion did not change the results for the Authoritarian Predisposition × College Degree interactions.
Use of the survey weights also computes standard errors using the linearized variance estimator, which provides a correction similar to that given by the Huber/White robust standard estimator in the context of regression without survey weights (Wolter 2007). Thus, the significance tests in our OLS models are protected against the heteroskedasticity implied by our second hypothesis.
To verify that the moderating effect of education was monotonic, we recoded education as a five- category ordinal variable: (1) less than a high-school degree, (2) high school degree only, (3) more than 12 years of schooling but no bachelor’s, (4) bachelor’s degree, and (5) advanced degree. This variable was then substituted for the college-degree indicator, and the model was re-run. CLARIFY (King et al. 2000) was then used to simulate conditional effects of the authoritarian predisposition at the five education levels. These estimates revealed a monotonic progression (from the lowest to the highest education group): −0.04, 0.03, 0.11, 0.18, and 0.25. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. This replicates our basic interaction effect.
CLARIFY estimates similar to those performed in 2004 for the same five education categories again revealed a monotonic progression of effect sizes (from lowest to highest education): 0.01, 0.03, 0.06, 0.08, and 0.10. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero.
In supplementary analyses, two-way Beliefs about Government × College Degree interactions are added to the models in Tables 2 and 3. Again, this specification allows the effect of beliefs about government to vary across education levels. These interactions failed to reach significance in any of the equations for the 2004 and 2008 data (ps > 0.10), and their inclusion did not change the results for the Authoritarian Predisposition × College Degree interactions.
To verify that the moderating effect of education was monotonic in the equation for the Republican versus independent comparison, we repeated our analysis with the five-category education measure described about and used CLARIFY to simulate first differences for the authoritarian predisposition in each category. Again, the first differences varied in a monotonic fashion (from lowest to highest education): -0.01, 0.06, 0.13, 0.21, and 0.29. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. The analysis for the Democratic versus independent comparison revealed no first differences whose confidence intervals excluded zero.
To verify that a monotonic moderating effect of education for the Republican versus independent comparison, we repeated our analysis with the five-category education measure described above for 2004. The first differences varied in a monotonic fashion (from lowest to highest education): 0.03, 0.05, 0.08, 0.10, and 0.14. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. A similar analysis for the Democratic versus independent comparison revealed no first differences whose confidence intervals excluded zero.
The pre and post information ratings were highly correlated (r = 0.73 in 2004, r = 0.67 in 2008).
The stronger effects of the authoritarian predisposition among the well-educated do not appear to be due to markedly greater reliability in the measure among those with college degrees (Cohen et al. 2003). In both datasets, the authoritarian predisposition had similar reliability in both education groups (i.e., α = 0.57 in the no-college group versus α = 0.60 in the college group in 2004; α = 0.65 in both groups in 2008).
Although our main focus is on education, the logic of our theory implies that other variables tapping attention to elite discourse should function similarly to education. To examine this, we repeated all of our analyses with interviewer ratings of respondents’ information levels substituted for education; we choose information since it is perhaps the best predictor of reception and understanding of elite signals (e.g., Zaller 1992). In 2004, this analysis revealed a significant Authoritarian Predisposition × Information interaction in an OLS model predicting the seven-point party measure (b = 0.27, p = 0.05). In the 2004 multinomial model, this interaction was significant in the Republican versus independent equation (b = 2.83, p < 0.05) but not the Democratic versus independent one (b = 1.10, p > 0.40). In 2008, this analysis revealed a significant Authoritarian Predisposition × Information interaction in an OLS model predicting the seven-point party measure (b = 0.34, p < 0.001). In the 2008 multinomial model, this interaction was significant in the Republican versus independent equation (b = 3.13, p < 0.001) but not the Democratic versus independent one (b = −0.29, p > 0.60). These analyses thus confirm and replicate our education results.
References
Abramowitz, A. I. (2010). The disappearing center: Engaged citizens, polarization, and American Democracy. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interaction. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other ‘authoritarian personality’. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 30 (pp. 47–92). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Barker, D. C., & Tinnick, J. D. (2006). Competing visions of parental roles and ideological constraint. American Political Science Review, 100, 249–263.
Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. London: Collier Macmillan.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.
Carmines, E. G., & Stimson, J. A. (1989). Issue evolution: Race and the transformation of American politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Christie, R. (1954). Authoritarianism re-examined. In R. Christie & M. Jahoda (Eds.), Studies in the scope and method of “the authoritarian personality”. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.
Delli Carpini, M. X. D., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters?. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41–113). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2009). A dual-process motivational model of ideological attitudes and system justification. In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.
Duriez, B., & Van Hiel, A. (2002). The march of modern fascism: A comparison of social dominance orientation and authoritarianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1199–1213.
Duriez, B., Van Hiel, A., & Kossowska, M. (2005). Authoritarianism and social dominance in western and eastern Europe: The importance of the socio-political context and of political interest and involvement. Political Psychology, 26, 299–320.
Federico, C. M. (2004). When do welfare attitudes become racialized? The paradoxical effects of education. American Journal of Political Science, 48, 374–391.
Federico, C. M. (2005). Racial perceptions and evaluative responses to welfare: Does education attenuate race-of-target effects? Political Psychology, 26, 683–698.
Federico, C. M., & Goren, P. (2009). Motivated social cognition and ideology: Is attention to elite discourse a prerequisite for epistemically motivated political affinities? In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.
Federico, C. M., & Deason, G. (2012). Uncertainty, insecurity, and ideological defense of the status quo: The extremitizing role of political expertise. In M. A. Hogg & D. L. Blaylock (Eds.), Extremism and the psychology of uncertainty. Boston: Wiley-Blackwell.
Federico, C. M., Fisher, E. L., & Deason, G. (2011). Political expertise and the link between the authoritarian predisposition and conservatism. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75, 686–708.
Federico, C. M., & Holmes, J. W. (2005). Education and the interface between racial attitudes and criminal-justice attitudes. Political Psychology, 26, 47–76.
Federico, C. M., Hunt, C. V., & Ergun, D. (2009). Political expertise, social worldviews, and ideology: Translating “competitive jungles” and “dangerous worlds” into ideological reality. Social Justice Research, 22, 259–279.
Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. C. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252.
Federico, C. M., & Sidanius, J. (2002). Racism, ideology, and affirmative action, revisited: The antecedents and consequences of ‘principled objections’ to affirmative action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 488–502.
Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24, 41–74.
Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 4, 741–770.
Freeman, J. (1986). The political culture of the Democratic and Republican Parties. Political Science Quarterly, 101, 327–356.
Gauchat, G. (2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77, 167–187.
Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2012). Personality and the strength and direction of partisan identification. Political Behavior, 34, 653–688.
Hetherington, M., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jennings, M. K. (1992). Ideological thinking among mass publics and political elites. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 419–441.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, function, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–338.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.
Kemmelmeier, M. (2007). Political conservatism, rigidity and dogmatism in American Foreign Policy officials: The 1966 Mennis data. Journal of Psychology, 141, 77–90.
Kemmelmeier, M. (2009). Authoritarianism and its relationship with intuitive-experiential cognitive style and heuristic processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 44–48.
Kinder, D. R. (2006). Politics and the life cycle. Science, 312, 1905–1908.
King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 347–361.
Kohn, M. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
Layman, G. (2001). The great divide: Religious and cultural conflict in American party politics. New York: Columbia University Press.
Layman, G., & Carsey, T. M. (2002). Party polarization and ‘conflict extension’ in the American electorate. American Journal of Political Science, 46, 786–802.
Layman, G., Carsey, T. M., Green, J. C., Herrera, R., & Cooperman, R. (2010). Activists and conflict extension in American Party politics. American Journal of Political Science, 104, 324–346.
Lipset, S. M. (1960). Political man. New York: Doubleday.
Lipsitz, L. (1965). Working class authoritarianism: A re-evaluation. American Sociological Review, 301, 103–109.
Long, J. Scott, & Freese, J. (2005). Regression models for categorical outcomes using stata (2nd ed.). College Station, TX: Stata Press.
Lupia, A., Krosnick, J. A., Luevano, P., DeBell, M., & Donakowski, D. (2009). User’s guide to the ANES 2008 time series study. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.
Mann, T., & Ornstein, N. (2012). It’s even worse than it looks: How the American Constitutional system collided with the new politics of extremism. New York: Basic Books.
Martin, J. G. (1964). The tolerant personality. Detroit: Wayne State University Press.
McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America: The dance of ideology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
McClosky, H., & Zaller, J. (1984). The American ethos: Public attitudes toward democracy and capitalism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1997). Congress: A political-economic history of roll call voting. New York: Oxford University Press.
Powell, G. B, Jr. (2000). Elections as instrument of democracy: Majoritarian and proportional visions. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Simpson, M. (1972). Authoritarianism and education: A comparative approach. Sociometry, 35, 223–234.
Sinclair, B. (2006). Party wars: Polarization and the politics of national policy making. Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press.
Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sniderman, P. M., & Piazza, T. (1993). The scar of race. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wolter, K. M. (2007). Introduction to variance estimation (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendix
Appendix
Measures from the 2004 and 2008 National Election Studies
Partisanship. v043114, v043114a, and v043115, in 2004; v083097, v083098a, and v083098b, in 2008.
Authoritarian predisposition. v045208-v045211, in 2004; v085158-v058161, in 2008.
Beliefs about government. v043136 in 2004; v083108x in 2008.
Interviewer-rated political information. v043403 and v045303 in 2004; v083303 and v085403 in 2008.
Demographic and control variables. Items used to construct these measures are as follows: age (v043250, in 2004; v081104, in 2004), region (v041203, in 2004; v081204, in 2008), income (v043293x, in 2004; v083249 in 2008), the Black and Latino dummy variables (v043299, in 2004; v083251a, in 2008), gender (v043411, in 2004; v081101, in 2008), religiosity (v043220, v043221, and v043223/v043224, in 2004; v083182, v083183, and v083185/v083186, in 2008), biblical literalism (v043222 in 2004; w083184 in 2008).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Federico, C.M., Tagar, M.R. Zeroing in on the Right: Education and the Partisan Expression of Authoritarianism in the United States. Polit Behav 36, 581–603 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9250-4
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9250-4