Skip to main content
Log in

Zeroing in on the Right: Education and the Partisan Expression of Authoritarianism in the United States

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we examine how the relationship between authoritarianism and partisanship is conditioned by education. Arguing against perspectives suggesting that authoritarianism is the province of the “unsophisticated,” we hypothesize that the relationship between education and Republican partisanship should be stronger among educated Americans. Moreover, going beyond previous work on how education may moderate the political impact of psychological dispositions, we also argue that partisan signals pertinent to authoritarianism come more strongly from the right, producing a pattern in which the positive relationship between authoritarianism and preference for the Republican Party over independence is stronger among the educated but the negative relationship between authoritarianism and preference for the Democratic Party over independence is not stronger among the educated. Data from the 2004 and 2008 American National Election Studies indicate clear support for both hypotheses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In supplementary analyses not shown here, all of the models reported below were also re-run with an additional interaction between religiosity and biblical literalism included; this was done to explore the possibility that religiosity predicts partisanship more among the religiously traditional. These interactions were not significant, and their inclusion did not change any of our primary results.

  2. In supplementary analyses, two-way Beliefs about Government × College Degree interactions are added to the models in Table 1. This specification allows the effect of beliefs about government as well as the effect of authoritarianism to vary across education levels. Although these interactions were significant in both 2004 and 2008 (ps < 0.001), their inclusion did not change the results for the Authoritarian Predisposition × College Degree interactions.

  3. Use of the survey weights also computes standard errors using the linearized variance estimator, which provides a correction similar to that given by the Huber/White robust standard estimator in the context of regression without survey weights (Wolter 2007). Thus, the significance tests in our OLS models are protected against the heteroskedasticity implied by our second hypothesis.

  4. To verify that the moderating effect of education was monotonic, we recoded education as a five- category ordinal variable: (1) less than a high-school degree, (2) high school degree only, (3) more than 12 years of schooling but no bachelor’s, (4) bachelor’s degree, and (5) advanced degree. This variable was then substituted for the college-degree indicator, and the model was re-run. CLARIFY (King et al. 2000) was then used to simulate conditional effects of the authoritarian predisposition at the five education levels. These estimates revealed a monotonic progression (from the lowest to the highest education group): −0.04, 0.03, 0.11, 0.18, and 0.25. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. This replicates our basic interaction effect.

  5. CLARIFY estimates similar to those performed in 2004 for the same five education categories again revealed a monotonic progression of effect sizes (from lowest to highest education): 0.01, 0.03, 0.06, 0.08, and 0.10. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero.

  6. In supplementary analyses, two-way Beliefs about Government × College Degree interactions are added to the models in Tables 2 and 3. Again, this specification allows the effect of beliefs about government to vary across education levels. These interactions failed to reach significance in any of the equations for the 2004 and 2008 data (ps > 0.10), and their inclusion did not change the results for the Authoritarian Predisposition × College Degree interactions.

  7. To verify that the moderating effect of education was monotonic in the equation for the Republican versus independent comparison, we repeated our analysis with the five-category education measure described about and used CLARIFY to simulate first differences for the authoritarian predisposition in each category. Again, the first differences varied in a monotonic fashion (from lowest to highest education): -0.01, 0.06, 0.13, 0.21, and 0.29. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. The analysis for the Democratic versus independent comparison revealed no first differences whose confidence intervals excluded zero.

  8. To verify that a monotonic moderating effect of education for the Republican versus independent comparison, we repeated our analysis with the five-category education measure described above for 2004. The first differences varied in a monotonic fashion (from lowest to highest education): 0.03, 0.05, 0.08, 0.10, and 0.14. The 95% confidence intervals for the top three categories did not contain zero. A similar analysis for the Democratic versus independent comparison revealed no first differences whose confidence intervals excluded zero.

  9. The pre and post information ratings were highly correlated (r = 0.73 in 2004, r = 0.67 in 2008).

  10. The stronger effects of the authoritarian predisposition among the well-educated do not appear to be due to markedly greater reliability in the measure among those with college degrees (Cohen et al. 2003). In both datasets, the authoritarian predisposition had similar reliability in both education groups (i.e., α = 0.57 in the no-college group versus α = 0.60 in the college group in 2004; α = 0.65 in both groups in 2008).

  11. Although our main focus is on education, the logic of our theory implies that other variables tapping attention to elite discourse should function similarly to education. To examine this, we repeated all of our analyses with interviewer ratings of respondents’ information levels substituted for education; we choose information since it is perhaps the best predictor of reception and understanding of elite signals (e.g., Zaller 1992). In 2004, this analysis revealed a significant Authoritarian Predisposition × Information interaction in an OLS model predicting the seven-point party measure (b = 0.27, p = 0.05). In the 2004 multinomial model, this interaction was significant in the Republican versus independent equation (b = 2.83, p < 0.05) but not the Democratic versus independent one (b = 1.10, p > 0.40). In 2008, this analysis revealed a significant Authoritarian Predisposition × Information interaction in an OLS model predicting the seven-point party measure (b = 0.34, p < 0.001). In the 2008 multinomial model, this interaction was significant in the Republican versus independent equation (b = 3.13, p < 0.001) but not the Democratic versus independent one (b = −0.29, p > 0.60). These analyses thus confirm and replicate our education results.

References

  • Abramowitz, A. I. (2010). The disappearing center: Engaged citizens, polarization, and American Democracy. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interaction. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other ‘authoritarian personality’. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 30 (pp. 47–92). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, D. C., & Tinnick, J. D. (2006). Competing visions of parental roles and ideological constraint. American Political Science Review, 100, 249–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. London: Collier Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmines, E. G., & Stimson, J. A. (1989). Issue evolution: Race and the transformation of American politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie, R. (1954). Authoritarianism re-examined. In R. Christie & M. Jahoda (Eds.), Studies in the scope and method of “the authoritarian personality”. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delli Carpini, M. X. D., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters?. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41–113). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2009). A dual-process motivational model of ideological attitudes and system justification. In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duriez, B., & Van Hiel, A. (2002). The march of modern fascism: A comparison of social dominance orientation and authoritarianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1199–1213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duriez, B., Van Hiel, A., & Kossowska, M. (2005). Authoritarianism and social dominance in western and eastern Europe: The importance of the socio-political context and of political interest and involvement. Political Psychology, 26, 299–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M. (2004). When do welfare attitudes become racialized? The paradoxical effects of education. American Journal of Political Science, 48, 374–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M. (2005). Racial perceptions and evaluative responses to welfare: Does education attenuate race-of-target effects? Political Psychology, 26, 683–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Goren, P. (2009). Motivated social cognition and ideology: Is attention to elite discourse a prerequisite for epistemically motivated political affinities? In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Deason, G. (2012). Uncertainty, insecurity, and ideological defense of the status quo: The extremitizing role of political expertise. In M. A. Hogg & D. L. Blaylock (Eds.), Extremism and the psychology of uncertainty. Boston: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., Fisher, E. L., & Deason, G. (2011). Political expertise and the link between the authoritarian predisposition and conservatism. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75, 686–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Holmes, J. W. (2005). Education and the interface between racial attitudes and criminal-justice attitudes. Political Psychology, 26, 47–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., Hunt, C. V., & Ergun, D. (2009). Political expertise, social worldviews, and ideology: Translating “competitive jungles” and “dangerous worlds” into ideological reality. Social Justice Research, 22, 259–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. C. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Sidanius, J. (2002). Racism, ideology, and affirmative action, revisited: The antecedents and consequences of ‘principled objections’ to affirmative action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 488–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24, 41–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 4, 741–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. (1986). The political culture of the Democratic and Republican Parties. Political Science Quarterly, 101, 327–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauchat, G. (2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77, 167–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2012). Personality and the strength and direction of partisan identification. Political Behavior, 34, 653–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, M. K. (1992). Ideological thinking among mass publics and political elites. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 419–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, function, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemmelmeier, M. (2007). Political conservatism, rigidity and dogmatism in American Foreign Policy officials: The 1966 Mennis data. Journal of Psychology, 141, 77–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemmelmeier, M. (2009). Authoritarianism and its relationship with intuitive-experiential cognitive style and heuristic processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 44–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R. (2006). Politics and the life cycle. Science, 312, 1905–1908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 347–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layman, G. (2001). The great divide: Religious and cultural conflict in American party politics. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layman, G., & Carsey, T. M. (2002). Party polarization and ‘conflict extension’ in the American electorate. American Journal of Political Science, 46, 786–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Layman, G., Carsey, T. M., Green, J. C., Herrera, R., & Cooperman, R. (2010). Activists and conflict extension in American Party politics. American Journal of Political Science, 104, 324–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipset, S. M. (1960). Political man. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipsitz, L. (1965). Working class authoritarianism: A re-evaluation. American Sociological Review, 301, 103–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, J. Scott, & Freese, J. (2005). Regression models for categorical outcomes using stata (2nd ed.). College Station, TX: Stata Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., Krosnick, J. A., Luevano, P., DeBell, M., & Donakowski, D. (2009). User’s guide to the ANES 2008 time series study. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, T., & Ornstein, N. (2012). It’s even worse than it looks: How the American Constitutional system collided with the new politics of extremism. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. G. (1964). The tolerant personality. Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America: The dance of ideology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClosky, H., & Zaller, J. (1984). The American ethos: Public attitudes toward democracy and capitalism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1997). Congress: A political-economic history of roll call voting. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, G. B, Jr. (2000). Elections as instrument of democracy: Majoritarian and proportional visions. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, M. (1972). Authoritarianism and education: A comparative approach. Sociometry, 35, 223–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, B. (2006). Party wars: Polarization and the politics of national policy making. Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Piazza, T. (1993). The scar of race. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wolter, K. M. (2007). Introduction to variance estimation (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher M. Federico.

Appendix

Appendix

Measures from the 2004 and 2008 National Election Studies

Partisanship. v043114, v043114a, and v043115, in 2004; v083097, v083098a, and v083098b, in 2008.

Authoritarian predisposition. v045208-v045211, in 2004; v085158-v058161, in 2008.

Beliefs about government. v043136 in 2004; v083108x in 2008.

Interviewer-rated political information. v043403 and v045303 in 2004; v083303 and v085403 in 2008.

Demographic and control variables. Items used to construct these measures are as follows: age (v043250, in 2004; v081104, in 2004), region (v041203, in 2004; v081204, in 2008), income (v043293x, in 2004; v083249 in 2008), the Black and Latino dummy variables (v043299, in 2004; v083251a, in 2008), gender (v043411, in 2004; v081101, in 2008), religiosity (v043220, v043221, and v043223/v043224, in 2004; v083182, v083183, and v083185/v083186, in 2008), biblical literalism (v043222 in 2004; w083184 in 2008).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Federico, C.M., Tagar, M.R. Zeroing in on the Right: Education and the Partisan Expression of Authoritarianism in the United States. Polit Behav 36, 581–603 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9250-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9250-4

Keywords

Navigation