Abstract
How do regular people define the term “political”? This original study gives Americans and Canadians an opportunity to express their interpretations of the concept. It identifies a great deal of inter-personal variation in terms of how many and what kinds of topics people perceive as the stuff of politics. And this variation comes in predictable patterns: the findings reveal correlations between socio-political attributes (such as gender, nationality and ideology) and the boundaries people draw around the political domain. The study also provides insight into the ways people distinguish the political from the non-political in their minds. And importantly, individuals’ interpretations of the term “politics” relate systematically to other measures of self-reported political behavior including political interest and frequency of political discussion. These results can be used to refine survey analysis and to broaden knowledge of day-to-day citizen politics.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
Examples of such questionnaire items include:
“How interested would you say you are in politics?” World Values Survey.
“When you get together with your friends, would you say you discuss political matters frequently, occasionally or never?” World Values Survey.
“How often do you read the political content of a newspaper?” International Social Science Programme.
“In political matters, people talk of ‘the left’ and ‘the right’. How would you place your views on this scale?” General Social Survey.
“Dis/Agree: I consider myself well-qualified to participate in politics.” American National Election Studies.
Additional examples of their connection include the Blue Water Beat (an online forum publicizing events on both sides of the border), the two-year Blue Water International Sculpture Exhibit that spans the entire area, the Seaway Sounds Chorus (a singing organization with members from both sides of the bridge), and the presence of local Sarnia radio and television stations on Port Huron dials (and vice versa).
Participants were invited to participate in a “Support your Symphony” survey; no details about the questionnaire were revealed in advance. The survey was administered over two consecutive days in two locations: in Port Huron on Saturday and Sunday mornings and in Sarnia on these afternoons.
Attention to the context created by the survey is in order. All participants were presented with the exact same item list, which could lead to concerns that a shorter list might have generated different outcomes. For instance, results might be influenced by floor or ceiling effects in that such a long list led respondents to think that some—but not all—items must be political. A conclusive response to this concern is not possible with the data here. However, two aspects of the study make this less of a pressing matter than it might otherwise be. First, this is not a representative sample and so it cannot speak beyond the present group of respondents to questions of what, exactly, “political” means in the minds of the masses. Therefore, the stakes of a particular item not being identified as political due to survey design are limited. Second, the main goal of this study is to identify heterogeneity across people in their views of the political. And since the same instrument was used for all participants, this is easier to identify. Still, future use of this instrument should include tests of the survey context’s influence. Another potential concern is that the ordering of items influences people’s choices. To test for this, a simple correlation between the percent of respondents who chose each item and its placement in the list was calculated, showing no significant relationship. Rank-ordering the items by column (assuming people worked downward by column), the correlation is −.05 (sig. = .762). Ranking them by row (assuming people worked from left to right by row), the correlation is −.03 (sig. = .882).
Two versions of the questionnaire facilitated some experimental manipulations, though they do not apply directly to the survey items at hand and they appear after all of the key variable items.
It does not fit the requirements for a poisson model as the variance is greater than the mean.
Exploratory factor analysis (using varimax rotation) identifies the underlying structures of respondents’ item selections. Because the topics listed in the editorial instrument are dichotomous, and because such data is not ideal for factor analysis, an additional component of the survey was integrated into this process. Directly following the topics listing, the survey reads, “Now, of these topics, please rank the three most political.” Responses to this prompt combine with the original variable into an ordered scale: 0 = not political, .5 = political, 1 = one of the most political topics. Topics that did not yield a full range of values (for instance, nobody chose diet pills as a “most” most political topic) were excluded from the factor analysis (omitted topics are italicized in Table 2), leaving 24 items. Given the sample size of 241, this provides an acceptable ratio between respondent and item of 10 to 1 (Everitt 1975; Preacher and MacCallum, 2002). The factor analysis results are in Appendix Table A2. The five reported factors were selected for this further analysis because they had Eigenvalues greater than 1 and at least three variables with loading scores over .5.
Content analysis of the media environment in the week preceding the survey event (conducted December 12–13) reveals that global warming was highly publicized. The 2009 United Nations Climate Change Conference took place from December 7–18. Front pages of two US newspapers and two Canadian papers were examined for coverage from December 5–11. The New York Times ran three front-page stories that related to the summit; The Port Huron Times Herald made no mention of it. In comparison, the Toronto Sun published six front-page stories about the conference and the Sarnia Observer ran an article that previewed a local public event planned for December 12 in support of the summit. The summit was big news in both countries and as Fig. 1 shows, it was rated the fourth most “political” topic. But it appeared to be bigger news in Canada. Perhaps as a result, Canadians were especially likely to consider global warming a political topic per Table 2.
Though not investigated here, it is likely the same person would draw on different sets of considerations depending, for instance, on the social or informational context.
Full interactive models are not presented in this paper; they are available from the author.
Vertical bars represent 95 % confidence intervals.
An interactive analysis for partisanship reflects similar patterns but does not achieve significance.
The survey participants were randomly assigned to a set of headlines depending on whether the day of their birth was an even or odd number.
The difference for conflict definers is statistically significant at the .95 level.
Importantly, political interest, attention, disagreement and discussion probably shape people’s conceptualizations of the political. As such, no directional, causal claims are advanced here.
References
Almond Gabriel, & Sydney Verba, (1963). The civic culture. Boston: Little Brown.
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Bambra, C. (2005). Cash Versus Services: “Worlds of Welfare” and the Decommodification of Cash Benefits and Health Care Services. Journal of Social Policy, 34(2), 195–213.
Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.
Bennett, L. L. M., & Bennett, S. E. (1989). The impact of socialization and political dispositions. American Politics Research, 17(1), 105–122.
Bennett, S. E., Flickinger, R. S., & Rhine, S. L. (2000). Political talk over here, over there, over time. British Journal of Political Science, 30(1), 99–119.
Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: a study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bolsen, T., & Fay Lomax Cook, (2008). Trends: public opinion on energy policy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(2), 364–388.
Bratton, M. (2010). Anchoring the “D-Word” in comparative survey research. Journal of Democracy, 21(4), 106–113.
Bratton, M., Mattes, R., & Gymah-Boadi, E. (2004). Public opinion, democracy and market reform in Africa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Burns, N., Schlozman, K. L., & Verba, S. (2001). The Private Roots of Public Action: Gender, Equality, and Political Participation. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: John Wiley.
Canache, Damarys (2012). Citizens’ Conceptualizations of Democracy: Structural Complexity, Substantive Content, and Political Significance. Comparative Political Studies Online First: January 24.
Canache, D., Mondak, J. J., & Seligson, M. A. (2001). Meaning and measurement in cross-national research on satisfaction with democracy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 65(4), 506–528.
Canes-Wrone, B., & Shotts, K. (2004). The conditional nature of presidential re-sponsiveness to public opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 48(4), 690–706.
Caruso, E. M., Mead, N. L., & Balcetis, E. (2009). Political partisanship influences perception of biracial candidates’ skin tone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(48), 20168–20173.
Clarke, H. D., Kornberg, A., MacLeod, J., & Scotto, T. (2005). Too close to call: political choice in Canada, 2004. Political Science and Politics, 38(2), 247–253.
Conover, P. J., Crewe, I. M., & Searing, D. D. (1991). The nature of citizenship in the United States and Great Britain. The Journal of Politics, 53(3), 800–832.
Conover, P. J., & Searing, D. D. (2002). Expanding the envelope: citizenship, contextual methodologies, and comparative political psychology. In J. H. Kuklinski (Ed.), Thinking about political psychology (pp. 89–114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Conover, P Johnston, & Searing, D. D. (2005). Everyday Political Talk. Acta Politica, 40, 269–283.
Conover, P. J., Searing, D. D., & Crewe, I. (2002). The deliberative potential of political discussion. British Journal of Political Science, 32(1), 21–62.
Conover, P. J., Searing, D. D., & Crewe, I. (2004). The elusive ideal of equal citizenship: political theory and political psychology in the United States and Great Britain. The Journal of Politics, 66(4), 1036–1068.
Converse, P. (1975). Public opinion and voting behavior. In F. Greenstein & N. Polsby (Eds.), Handbook of political science: nongovernmental politics. Reading: Addison-Wesley.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: design and analysis issues for field settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Cook, F. L., Michael, X. D. C., & RJ, Lawrence. (2007). Who deliberates? Discursive participation in America. In S. W. Rosenberg (Ed.), Deliberation, participation and democracy: can the people govern?. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Curtis, J., & Lambert, R. (1976). Voting, election interest, and age: national findings for English and French Canadians. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 9(2), 293–307.
Dalton, R. J., Beck, P. A., & Huckfeldt, R. (1998). Partisan cues and the media. American Political Science Review, 92(1), 111–126.
Dalton, R. J., Shin, D. C., & Jou, W. (2007). Understanding democracy: data form unlikely places. Journal of Democracy, 18(4), 142–156.
Davidov, E., Schmidt, P., & Billiet, J. (2011). Cross-cultural analysis: methods and applications. New York: Routledge.
Delli Carpini, M., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Eliasoph, N. (1998). Avoiding politics: how americans produce apathy in everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Esping-Andersen, G. (1990). The three worlds of welfare capitalism. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Everitt, B. S. (1975). Multivariate analysis: the need for data, and other problems. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 237–240.
Gamson, W. A. (2002). Talking politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Glenn, N. D., & Grimes, M. (1968). Aging, voting, and political interest. American Sociological Review, 33(4), 563–575.
Hall, P. A., & Soskice, D. (Eds.). (2001). Varieties of capitalism: the institutional foundations of comparative advantage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hansen, S. B. (1997). Talking about politics: gender and contextual effects on political proselytizing. Journal of Politics, 59(1), 73–103.
Hayes, B. C., & Bean, C. S. (1993). Gender and local political interest: some international comparisons. Political Studies, 41(4), 672–682.
Hibbing, M. V., Ritchie, M., & Anderson, M. R. (2011). Personality and political discussion. Political Behavior, 33(4), 601–624.
Hooghe, M., & Stolle, D. (2004). Good girls go to the polling booth, bad boys go everywhere. Women & Politics, 26(3–4), 1–23.
Huckfeldt, R. (2007). Unanimity, discord, and the communication of public opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 978–995.
Huckfeldt, R., Beck, P. A., Dalton, R. J., & Levine, J. (1995). Political environments, cohesive social groups, and the communication of public opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 39(4), 1025–1054.
Huckfeldt, R., & Mendez, J. M. (2008). Moths, flames, and political engagement: managing disagreement within communication networks. The Journal of Politics, 70(1), 83–96.
Inglehart, M. L. (1981). Political interest in West European Women: an historical and empirical comparative analysis. Comparative Political Studies, 14(3), 299–326.
Iyengar, S. (1990). Framing responsibility for political issues: the case of poverty. Political Behavior, 12(1), 19–40.
Jennings, M. K. (1983). Gender roles and inequalities in political participation: results from and eight-nation study. Western Political Quarterly, 36(3), 364–385.
Jones-Correa, M. A., & Leal, D. L. (2001). Political participation: does religion matter? Political Research Quarterly December, 54(4), 751–770.
Jowell, R., Roberts, C., F, R., & Eva, G. (2007). Measuring attitudes cross-nationally: lessons from the European Social Survey. London: Sage.
Kenny, C. B. (1992). Political participation and effects from the social environment. American Journal of Political Science, 36(1), 259–267.
Kershaw, P. (2004). ‘choice’ discourse in BC Child Care: distancing Policy from Research. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 37(4), 927–950.
King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 341–355.
Klofstad, C. A. (2009). civic talk and civic participation: the moderating effect of individual predispositions. American Politics Research, 37(5), 856–878.
Kuklinski, James H., and Norman L. Hurley (1996). It’s a Matter of Interpretation. Ch. 5 in Diana C. Mutz, Paul M. Sniderman, Richard A Brody, eds., Political Persuasion and Attitude Change. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Kwak, N., Williams, A. E., Wang, X., & Lee, H. (2005). Talking politics and engaging politics. Communication Research, 32(1), 87–111.
La Due Lake, R., & Huckfeldt, R. (1998). Social Capital, Social Networks, and Political Participation. Political Psychology, 19(3), 567–584.
Lane, R. E. (1962). Political Ideology: Why the American Common Man Believes What He Does. Glencoe: The Free Press of Glencoe.
Lau, R. R., Sears, D. O., & Jessor, T. (1990). Fact or artifact revisited: survey instrument effects and pocketbook politics. Political Behavior, 12(3), 217–242.
Lodge, M., & Hamill, Ruth. (1986). A partisan schema for political information processing. The American Political Science Review, 80(2), 505–520.
Lupia, A., & Philpot, T. S. (2005). Views from inside the net: how websites affect young adults’ political Interest. Journal of Politics, 67(4), 1122–1142.
Mondak, J. J. (1993). Public opinion and heuristic processing of source cues. Political Behavior, 15(2), 167–192.
Mondak, J. J., Hibbing, M. V., Canache, D., Seligson, M. A., & Anderson, M. R. (2010). Personality and civic engagement: an integrative framework for the study of trait effects on political behavior. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 85–110.
Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: deliberative versus participatory democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nisbet, M. C. (2005). The competition for worldviews: values, information and public support for stem cell research. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 17(1), 90–112.
Nisbet, M. C., & Myers, T. (2007). Trends: twenty years of public opinion about global warming. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(3), 444–470.
Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1983). Effects of public opinion on policy. American Political Science Review, 77(1), 175–190.
Preacher, K. J., & MacCallum, R. C. (2002). Exploratory factor analysis in behavior genetics research: factor recovery with small sample sizes. Behavior Genetics, 32, 153–161.
Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making democracy work: civic traditions in modern Italy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Reeskens, T., & Hooghe, M. (2008). Cross-cultural measurement equivalence of generalized trust. Evidence from the European Social Survey (2002 and 2004). Social Indicators Research, 85, 515–532.
Ridout, T. N., Shah, D. V., Goldstein, K. M., & Franz, M. M. (2004). Evaluating measures of campaign advertising exposure on political learning. Political Behavior, 26(3), 201–225.
Rosenstone, S. J., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and American democracy. New York: MacMillan.
Sears, D. O., & Valentino, N. A. (1997). Politics matters: political events as catalysts for preadult socialization. American Political Science Review, 91(1), 45–65.
Seligson, M. (2004). Comparative survey research: is there a problem? APSA-Comparative Politics Newsletter, 15, 11–14.
Stolle, D., & Gidengil, E. (2010). What do women really know? a gendered analysis of varieties of political knowledge. Perspectives on Politics, 8(1), 93–109.
Straits, B. C. (1991). Bringing strong ties back in: interpersonal gateways to political information and influence. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(3), 432–448.
Taber, C. S., Cann, D., & Kucsova, S. (2009). The motivated processing of political arguments. Political Behavior, 31(1), 137–155.
Verba, S., Burns, N., & Schlozman, K. L. (1997). Knowing and caring about politics: gender and political engagement. Journal of Politics, 59(4), 1051–1072.
Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality: civic voluntarism in American politics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Walsh, K. C. (2004). Talking about politics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Wyatt, R. O., Katz, E., & Kim, J. (2000). Bridging the spheres: political and personal conversation in public and private spaces. Journal of Communication, 50, 71–92.
Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Acknowledgments
This research was made possible by an Innovative Seed Grant Award from the Vice Chancellor of Research at the University of Colorado at Boulder. The author thanks Ceren Altincekic, Vanessa Baird, Andy Baker, Anne Brown and the ISO, Hilde Coffé, Amber Curtis, Fay Fitzgerald, Dawn Kindsvater, Anand Sokhey and Jennifer Wolak for their various contributions to this project.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Fitzgerald, J. What does “Political” Mean to You?. Polit Behav 35, 453–479 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-012-9212-2
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-012-9212-2