Skip to main content
Log in

Public Accountability and Political Participation: Effects of a Face-to-Face Feedback Intervention on Voter Turnout of Public Housing Residents

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Low turnout among the urban poor has implications for democratic representation. The fact that turnout among the economically disadvantaged is especially low in municipal elections means that citizens most in need of services provided at the local level may not be represented in policy decisions that affect their daily lives. This paper reports the results of an experiment that compares the effects of two voter mobilization interventions: traditional canvassing appeals and face-to-face exchanges in which canvassers distribute a feedback intervention consisting of printed records of individual voter histories. In contrast to previous studies, this experiment measures the effectiveness of using social pressure to mobilize turnout among relatively infrequent voters in a low salience election. The campaign was implemented by a credible tenant advocacy organization within the context of a municipal election; the sample consisted of registered voters in two Boston public housing developments. I find that the feedback intervention dramatically increased voter turnout. Turnout among those reached by canvassers with voter histories was approximately 15–18 percentage points higher than turnout in the control group, an effect that is approximately 10 percentage points larger in magnitude than that of standard face-to-face mobilization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For example, see research on the effects of higher rates of turnout on election outcomes (DeNardo 1980; Nagel and McNulty 1996; Tucker et al. 1986), or margins of victory (Citrin et al. 2003).

  2. There is a debate about the extent to which participation in public assistance programs galvanizes or depresses political participation among recipients. For a review of theories relating the receipt of public assistance to one’s level of political engagement and participation, see Soss (1999) or Mettler and Soss (2004).

  3. Edna Carrasco of CBPH provided background information about the scope and work of the organization as well as contextual information related to the issues facing tenants of the public housing developments included in the sample.

  4. Information related to the approximate demographic composition of the housing developments and surrounding neighborhoods was provided by Edna Carrasco of CBPH through interviews and personal correspondence. Demographic attributes of the sample were obtained from voter files and the voter information database used by the organization.

  5. A large portion of Complex A was demolished after the study was conducted and residents were relocated to a different public housing development.

  6. Standard errors of the multinomial regressions are clustered by the unit of randomization, apartment entrance.

  7. Pew Research Center for People and the Press. June 2008. “Voter Attitudes Survey,” accessed online at http://people-press.org/reports/questionnaires/436.pdf on December 20, 2008.

  8. Contact rates were statistically the same in both treatment groups as indicated in Table 2. Results of probit regressions of contact on treatment assignment among subjects assigned to either treatment group (N = 540), with and without covariates for demographic attributes and previous turnout, imply no statistically distinguishable relationship between contact and the assignment to one treatment group relative to the other. Furthermore, restricting the sample to individuals who were successfully contacted by canvassers, results from probit analysis in which contact by one treatment is regressed on demographic and turnout covariates imply that covariates cannot predict which script contacted subjects heard (χ2(13) = 12.12; p-value = 0.52).

  9. In general, binary response models are considered to be more appropriate for estimation of effects of explanatory variable on binary dependent variables. However, for models in which the explanatory variables are all or mostly discrete and take on few values, as in the case of this analysis, in which all but one explanatory variable is binary, “the [linear probability model] need not provide very good estimates of partial effects at extreme values of” the explanatory variables (Wooldridge 2001, pp 455). For this reason, OLS and probit estimates do not differ substantially in the majority of the results.

  10. Canvassers recorded the results of their contact attempts on walk lists. I define contact as an encounter in which the canvasser has an interaction with the targeted subject. I do not count messages left with another member of the household as a successful contact.

  11. The basic model of the instrumental variables approach to estimating the difference between the canvassing and treatment effects may be specified as follows:

    $$ {\text{P}}\left\{ {{\text{Y}}_{\text{ia}} = 1} \right\} = \eta + \mu_{ 1} {\text{X}}_{{ 1 {\text{ia}}}} + \mu_{ 2} {\text{X}}_{{ 2 {\text{ia}}}} + \nu_{{ 1 {\text{ia}}}} $$
    (8)
    $$ {\text{P}}\{ {\text{ X}}_{{ 1 {\text{ia }}}} = 1\} = \varsigma + \varphi_{ 1} {\text{T}}_{\text{ia}} + \varphi_{ 2} {\text{H}}_{\text{ia}} + \nu ^{\prime}_{{ 2 {\text{ia}}}} $$
    (9)
    $$ {\text{P}}\{ {\text{ X}}_{{ 2 {\text{ia }}}} = 1 { }\} = \sigma + \psi_{ 1} {\text{T}}_{\text{ia}} + \psi_{ 2} {\text{H}}_{\text{ia}} + \nu ^{\prime \prime}_{{ 3 {\text{ia}}}} $$
    (10)

    In the preceding system of equations, Yia is a binary variable equal to one if subject i voted in the November 2007 election. X1ia is a binary variable equal to one if subject i was successfully contacted with either treatment, and X2ia is a binary indicator equal to one if subject i was contacted with the voter history feedback intervention. Tia is a binary variable equal to one if subject i was assigned to either treatment group, and Hia is a binary variable equal to one if the subject was assigned to the treatment group in which canvassers presented subjects individual voter histories, the feedback intervention. In the full model, covariates for past turnout and demographic attributes are added as specified in Eq. 2.

  12. The largest estimated treatment effect in the present analysis, the effect of actually receiving one’s record of previous turnout, modeled with covariates for previous voter turnout and demographic attributes, is 18.7 percentage points. This estimated effect, while it is substantial in magnitude, implies that in order to generate 1,000 additional votes, the organization would have to successfully contact 5,348 voters with voter histories. In the contested race of November 2007, the at-large city council contest, the four candidates who received the most votes won seats. The fourth place winner, John Connolly, received 21,997 votes, while the fifth place candidate (first runner-up), incumbent Felix Arroyo, received 18,579 votes. Felix Arroyo, the incumbent who lost his seat, was the only Latino ever elected to the city council. The next-closest candidate in the race received only 4,008 votes. The most likely scenario in which higher turnout could have influenced the outcome of the election would have been one in which approximately 3,900 more supporters of Felix Arroyo turned out to vote resulting in a margin of victory for fourth place of about 480 votes, which would be sufficient to avoid a recount. Turnout was particularly low in predominately Latino precincts relative to previous municipal elections. Conceivably, a very large mobilization campaign targeting Latino voters could have impacted the outcome of the election. The 18.7 percentage point local average treatment effect implies that a campaign would have to have successfully contacted 20,855 voters using voter histories, all of whom would have to vote for Felix Arroyo (a strong assumption) in order to have altered the outcome of the election. The contact rate of 37.7% among the feedback intervention experimental group implies that such a campaign would have to target 55,320 voters to obtain a sufficient number of contacts.

References

  • Alex-Assenhoh, Y., & Assenoh, A. B. (2001). Inner city contexts, church attendance, and African-American political participation. The Journal of Politics, 63, 886–901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alford, R. F., & Lee, E. C. (1968). Voting turnout in American cities. The American Political Science Review, 62, 796–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allison, P. D. (1992). The cultural evolution of beneficent norms. Social Forces, 71, 279–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (2008). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the new gilded age. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M., Heclo, H., Hero, R. E., & Jacobs, L. R. (2005). Inequality and American governance. In L. R. Jacobs & T. Skocpol (Eds.), Inequality and American democracy: What we know and what we need to learn (pp. 88–155). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. J. (2005). The perverse consequences of electoral reform in the United States. American Politics Research, 33, 471–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, R., Chadha, A., & Montjoy, R. (2001). Overreporting voting: Why it happens and why it matters. Public Opinion Quarterly, 65, 22–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blais, A. (2000). To vote or not to vote: The merits and limits of rational choice theory. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boston Globe. November 8, 2007. CPR for Boston politics. Editorial. pp. A.10.

  • Brady, H. E., Schlozman, K. L., & Verba, S. (1999). Prospecting for participants: Rational expectations and the recruitment of political activists. The American Political Science Review, 93, 153–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridges, A. (1997). Textbook municipal reform. Urban Affairs Review, 33, 97–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M., Soroka, S., Gonzago, K., Murphy, E., & Somervell, E. (2001). The effect of fleeting attraction on compliance to requests. Personal. Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1578–1586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caren, N. (2007). Big city, big turnout? Electoral participation in American cities. Journal of Urban Affairs, 29, 31–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, W. K., Tam, G., James, G., & Dyck, J. J. (2006). Residential concentration, political socialization, and voter turnout. Journal of Politics, 68, 156–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 30–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 151–192). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Citrin, J., Schickler, E., & Sides, J. (2003). What if everyone voted? Simulating the impact of increased turnout in senate elections. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, C. J., & Dawson, M. C. (1993). Neighborhood poverty and African American politics. The American Political Science Review, 87, 286–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. (2004). The effect of social conformity on collective voting behavior. Political Analysis, 12, 76–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeNardo, J. (1980). Turnout and the vote: The joke’s on the democrats. American Political Science Review, 74, 406–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, J. C. (2008, April 3). City Council considers moving voting day to Saturdays; Aim is to boost turnout in ‘09 mayoral race. Boston Globe, p. B.3.

  • Editorial. (2007, November 8). CPR for Boston politics. Boston Globe, p. A.10.

  • Eldersveld, S. J. (1956). Experimental propaganda techniques and voting behavior. American Political Science Review, 50, 154–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaudine, A. P., & Saks, A. M. (2001). Effects of an absenteeism feedback intervention on employment absence behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 15–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2000). The effects of canvassing, direct mail, and telephone contact on voter turnout: A field experiment. American Political Science Review, 94, 653–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer. C. W. (Forthcoming). An experiment testing the relative effectiveness of encouraging voter participation by inducing feelings of pride or shame. Political Behavior.

  • Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout: Evidence from a field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102, 33–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gimpel, J. G., Dyck, J. J., & Shaw, D. R. (2004). Registrants, voters, and turnout variability across neighborhoods. Political Behavior, 26, 343–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosnell, H. F. (1927). Getting-out-the-vote: An experiment in the stimulation of voting. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. P., & Gerber, A. S. (2008). Get out the vote: How to increase voter turnout (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, J. D., & Newman, B. (2005). Are voters better represented? The Journal of Politics, 64, 1206–1227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hajnal, Z. L., & Lewis, P. G. (2003). Municipal institutions and voter turnout in local elections. Urban Affairs Review, 38, 645–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajnal, Z., & Trounstine, J. (2005). Where turnout matters: The consequences of uneven turnout in city politics. The Journal of Politics, 67, 515–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh, W. T. (1996). If people vote because they like to, then why do so many of them lie? Public Choice, 89, 63–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, D., & Shaffer, M. (1994). Comparative examinations of self-reports and perceived absenteeism norms: Wading through Lake Wobegon. Journal f Applied Psychology, 79, 240–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huckfeldt, R., & Sprague, J. (1995). Citizens, politics, and social communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Karnig, A., & Walter, B. O. (1983). Decline in municiple voter turnout: A function of changing structure. American Politics Quarterly, 11, 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Key, V. O., Jr. (1949). Southern politics in state and nation. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knack, S., & Kropf, M. E. (1998). For shame! The effect of community cooperative context on the probability of voting. Political Psychology, 19, 585–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapinski, M. K., & Boster, F. J. (2001). Modeling the ego-defensive function of attitudes. Communication Monographs, 68, 314–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapinski, M. K., & Rimal, R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15, 127–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, J. L., & Fox, R. L. (2001). Political participation of the urban poor. Social Problems, 48, 362–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. (1995). Self presentation: Impression management and interpersonal behavior. Madison: Brown and Benchmark.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis-Beck, M. S., Jacoby, W. G., Norpoth, H., & Weisberg, H. F. (2008). The American voter revisited. AnnArbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manza, J., & Uggen, C. (2006). Locked out: Felon disenfranchisement and American democracy. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. (2003). Voting’s reward: Voter turnout, attentive publics, and congressional allocation of federal money. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 110–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massey, D. S., & Denton, N. (1993). American apartheid: Segregation and the making of the underclass. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettler, S., & Soss, J. (2004). The consequences of public policy for democratic citizenship: Bridging policy studies and mass politics. Perspectives on Politics, 2, 55–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morlan, R. L. (1984). Municipal versus national election voter turnout: Europe and the United States. Political Science Quarterly, 99, 457–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, J. H., & McNulty, J. E. (1996). Partisan effects of voter turnout in senatorial and gubernatorial elections. American Political Science Review, 90, 780–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nie, N. H., Junn, J., & Stehlik-Barry, K. (1996). Education and democratic citizenship. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piven, F. F., & Cloward, R. A. (1988). Why Americans don’t vote. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenstone, S. J., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization. New York: MacMillan Publishing Company, Participation, and Democracy in America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlozman, K. L., Page, B. I., Verba, S., & Fiorina, M. P. (2005). Inequalities of political voice. In L. R. Jacobs & T. Skocpol (Eds.), Inequality and American democracy: What we know and what we need to learn (pp. 19–87). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: A field experiment on curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 25–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, B. D., Anderson, B. A., & Abramson, P. R. (1986). Who overreports voting? The American Political Science Review, 80, 613–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soss, J. (1999). Lessons of welfare: Policy design, political learning, and political action. The American Political Science Review, 93, 363–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soss, J., & Jacobs, L. R. (2009). The place of inequality: Nonparticipation in the American polity. Political Science Quarterly, 24, 95–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, H. J., Vedlitz, A., & DeNardo, J. (1986). Does heavy turnout help democrats in presidential elections? American Political Science Review, 84, 1291–1298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfinger, R. E., & Rosenstone, S. J. (1980). Who votes? New haven.  : Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wooldridge, J. M. (2001). Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data (pp. 455–457). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author is grateful to the Institution for Social and Policy Studies at Yale and the Boston Foundation, both of which funded components of this research but bear no responsibility for the content of this report, Donald P. Green for guidance and support, Edna Carrasco and the staff of the Committee for Boston Public Housing for expertly implementing this experiment and providing background information about the work of the organization and its members, Avi Green, David Ortiz, and the staff of MassVOTE, Roxan McKinnon and Kathy Brown of the Boston Tenant Coalition, Bob LeLievre for voter database support, Dan Butler, Ken Scheve, Ryan Enos, and the editors and anonymous reviewers of Political Behavior for all of their helpful comments at various stages of the paper. This research was reviewed and approved by the Yale Human Subjects Committee.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tiffany C. Davenport.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Canvassing Appeal and Election Day Reminder Scripts

Appendix 2: Sample Voter History

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Davenport, T.C. Public Accountability and Political Participation: Effects of a Face-to-Face Feedback Intervention on Voter Turnout of Public Housing Residents. Polit Behav 32, 337–368 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9109-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9109-x

Keywords

Navigation